Advertisement

Intra- and interindividual differences in the within-person coupling between daily pain and affect of older adults

  • Marko KatanaEmail author
  • Christina Röcke
  • Mathias Allemand
Article
  • 24 Downloads

Abstract

The bidirectional interplay between chronic pain and negative affect is well-established in patient samples. However, less is known about the day-to-day relationship between pain and affect of older adults without severe illnesses and to what extent this association differs within and between individuals. A total of 224 participants (Mage = 77.6, SDage = 6.2) reported their daily experience of pain, impairment by their pain and affect during 21 consecutive days. Multilevel modeling results showed that on days with increased pain individuals also reported less positive affect and more negative affect. Time-lagged results indicated a temporal carry-over from yesterday’s pain to today’s negative affect but not to today’s positive affect. Moreover, on days when individuals reported stronger impairment by their pain, they showed a stronger within-person coupling between daily pain and affect in contrast to days with a weaker experience of daily impairment. Yesterday’s pain and today’s negative affect were more strongly associated within individuals who reported higher levels of impairment. Interindividual differences in the within-person coupling between daily pain and affect were found with regard to general physical health conditions and general satisfaction with health. This study demonstrated the importance of focusing on within-person couplings between daily pain and affect beyond patient samples in order to better understand the maintenance of emotional stability despite daily hassles in older adults’ everyday lives.

Keywords

Daily affect Pain Interindividual differences Older adults 

Notes

Acknowledgements

Work on the manuscript was supported by a grant from the Jacobs Foundation to the first author and by a grant from the Swiss National Science Foundation (SNSF No. 159349) to the last author. During the work on his dissertation, Marko Katana was a pre-doctoral fellow of the International Max Planck Research School on the Life Course (LIFE, www.imprs-life.mpg.de; participating institutions: Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Freie Universität Berlin, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, University of Michigan, University of Virginia, University of Zurich). The authors wish to thank Barbara Dudli for collecting the data.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

Marko Katana, Christina Röcke and Mathias Allemand declare that they do not have any conflict of interest.

Human and animal rights and informed consent

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Supplementary material

10865_2019_99_MOESM1_ESM.docx (51 kb)
Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 51 kb)

References

  1. Adolf, J. K., Voelkle, M. C., Brose, A., & Schmiedek, F. (2017). Capturing context-related change in emotional dynamics via fixed moderated time series analysis. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 52, 499–531.  https://doi.org/10.1080/00273171.2017.1321978 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Affleck, G., Tennen, H., Urrows, S., & Higgins, P. (1991). Individual differences in the day-to-day experience of chronic pain: A prospective daily study of rheumatoid arthritis patients. Health Psychology, 70, 419–426.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.10.6.419 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Affleck, G., Tennen, H., Urrows, S., & Higgins, P. (1994). Person and contextual features of daily stress reactivity: Individual differences in relations of undesirable daily events with mood disturbance and chronic pain intensity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 329–340.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.66.2.329 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Affleck, G., Urrows, S., Tennen, H., Higgins, P., & Abeles, M. (1996). Sequential daily relations of sleep, pain intensity, and attention to pain among women with fibromyalgia. Pain, 68, 363–368.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3959(96)03226-5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Baltes, P. B., & Smith, J. (2003). New frontiers in the future of aging: From successful aging of the young old to the dilemmas of the fourth age. Gerontology, 49, 123–135.  https://doi.org/10.1159/000067946 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., & Vohs, K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychology, 5, 323–370.  https://doi.org/10.1037//1089-2680.5.4.323 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bolger, N., & Laurenceau, J. P. (2013). Intensive longitudinal methods. New York: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  8. Brunstein, J. C. (1999). Persönliche ziele und subjektives wohlbefinden bei älteren menschen [personal goals and subjective well-being of elderly people]. Zeitschrift Für Differentielle Und Diagnostische Psychologie, 20, 58–71.  https://doi.org/10.1024//0170-1789.20.1.58 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Carstensen, L. L., Turan, B., Scheibe, S., Ram, N., Ersner-Hershfield, H., Samanez-Larkin, G. R., et al. (2011). Emotional experience improves with age: Evidence based on over 10 years of experience sampling. Psychology and Aging, 26, 21–33.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021285 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Cassidy, E. L., Atherton, R. J., Robertson, N., Walsh, D. A., & Gillett, R. (2012). Mindfulness, functioning and catastrophizing after multidisciplinary pain management for chronic low back pain. Pain, 153, 644–650.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2011.11.027 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Charles, S. T. (2010). Strength and vulnerability integration: A model of emotional well-being across adulthood. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 1068–1091.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021232 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Charles, S. T., & Almeida, D. M. (2006). Daily reports of symptoms and negative affect: Not all symptoms are the same. Psychology and Health, 21, 1–17.  https://doi.org/10.1080/14768320500129239 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Charles, S. T., & Piazza, J. R. (2009). Age differences in affective well-being: Context matters. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 5, 711–724.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2009.00202.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Connelly, M., Keefe, F. J., Affleck, G., Lumley, M. A., Anderson, T., & Waters, S. (2007). Effects of day-to-day affect regulation on the pain experience of patients with rheumatoid arthritis. Pain, 131, 162–170.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2007.01.002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Crombez, G., Viane, I., Eccleston, C., Devulder, J., & Goubert, L. (2013). Attention to pain and fear of pain in patients with chronic pain. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 36, 371–378.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10865-012-9433-1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Day, M. A., Jensen, M. P., Ehde, D. M., & Thorn, B. E. (2014). Toward a theoretical model for mindfulness-based pain management. Journal of Pain, 15, 691–703.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2014.03.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Dezutter, J., Luyckx, K., & Wachholtz, A. (2015). Meaning in life in chronic pain patients over time: Associations with pain experience and psychological well-being. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 38, 384–396.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10865-014-9614-1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Diefenbach, M. A., Leventhal, E. A., Leventhal, H., & Patrick-Miller, L. (1996). Negative affect relates to cross-sectional but not longitudinal symptom reporting: Data from elderly adults. Health Psychology, 4, 282–288.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.15.4.282 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Dobscha, S. K., Morasco, B. J., Kovas, A. E., Peters, D. M., Hart, K., & McFarland, B. H. (2015). Short-term variability in outpatient pain intensity scores in a national sample of older veterans with chronic pain. Pain Medicine, 16, 855–865.  https://doi.org/10.1111/pme.12266 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Eriksen, H. R., & Ursin, H. (2004). Subjective health complaints, sensitization, and sustained cognitive activation (stress). Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 56, 445–448.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-3999(03)00629-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Feldman, S. I., Downey, G., & Schaffer-Neitz, R. (1999). Pain, negative mood, and perceived support in chronic pain patients: A daily diary study of people with reflex sympathetic dystrophy syndrome. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67, 776–785.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.67.5.776 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Finan, P. H., Quartana, P. J., & Smith, M. T. (2013). Positive and negative affect dimensions in chronic knee osteoarthritis. Psychosomatic Medicine, 75, 463–470.  https://doi.org/10.1097/psy.0b013e31828ef1d6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Freund, A. M., & Riediger, M. (2003). Successful aging. In R. M. Lerner, M. A. Easterbrooks, & J. Mistry (Eds.), Handbook of psychology (pp. 601–628). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  24. Gatchel, R. J., Peng, Y. B., Peters, M. L., Fuchs, P. N., & Turk, D. C. (2007). The biopsychosocial approach to chronic pain: Scientific advances and future directions. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 581–624.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.133.4.581 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Gelman, A. (2006). Multilevel (hierarchical) modeling: What it can and cannot do. Technometrics, 48, 432–435.  https://doi.org/10.1198/004017005000000661 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Gerstorf, D., Ram, N., Mayraz, G., Hidajat, M., Lindenberger, U., Wagner, G. G., et al. (2010). Late-life decline in well-being across adulthood in Germany, the United Kingdom, and the United States: Something is seriously wrong at the end of life. Psychology and Aging, 25, 477–485.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017543 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Gerstorf, D., Ram, N., Röcke, C., Lindenberger, U., & Smith, J. (2008). Decline in life satisfaction in old age: Longitudinal evidence for links to distance from death. Psychology and Aging, 23, 154–168.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.23.1.154 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Ghisletta, P., Bickel, J., & Lövdén, M. (2006). Does activity engagement protect against cognitive decline in old age? Methodological and analytical considerations. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 61B, 253–261.  https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/61.5.P253 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Gibson, S. J., & Lussier, D. (2012). Prevalence and relevance of pain in older persons. Pain Medicine, 13, 23–26.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1526-4637.2012.01349.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Gil, K. M., Carson, J. W., Porter, L. S., Scipio, C., Bediako, S. M., & Orringer, E. (2004). Daily mood and stress predict pain, health care use, and work activity in african american adults with sickle-cell disease. Health Psychology, 23, 267–274.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.23.3.267 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Graham-Engeland, J. E., Zawadzki, M. J., Slavish, D. C., & Smyth, J. M. (2016). Depressive symptoms and momentary mood predict momentary pain among rheumatoid arthritis patients. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 50, 12–23.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-015-9723-2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Green, A. S., Rafaeli, E., Bolger, N., Shrout, P. E., & Reis, H. T. (2006). Paper or plastic? Data equivalence in paper and electronic diaries. Psychological Methods, 11, 87–105.  https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.11.1.87 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Gruenenfelder-Steiger, A. E., Katana, M., Martin, A. A., Aschwanden, D., Koska, J. L., Kündig, Y., et al. (2017). Physical activity and depressive mood in the daily life of older adults. GeroPsych, 30, 119–129.  https://doi.org/10.1024/1662-9647/a000172 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Hamilton, N. A., Zautra, A. J., & Reich, J. W. (2005). Affect and pain in rheumatoid arthritis: Do individual differences in affective regulation and affective intensity predict emotional recovery from pain? Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 29, 216–224.  https://doi.org/10.1207/s15324796abm2903_8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Hanssen, M. M., Vancleef, L. M. G., Vlaeyen, J. W. S., & Peters, M. L. (2014). More optimism, less pain! The influence of generalized and pain-specific expectations on experienced cold-pressor pain. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 37, 47–58.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10865-012-9463-8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Hoffman, L. (2007). Multilevel models for examining individual differences in within-person variation and covariation over time. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 42, 609–629.  https://doi.org/10.1080/00273170701710072 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Hülür, G., Hoppmann, C. A., Ram, N., & Gerstorf, D. (2015). Developmental associations between short-term variability and long-term changes: Intraindividual correlation of positive and negative affect in daily life and cognitive aging. Developmental Psychology, 51, 987–997.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0039341 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Idler, E. L., & Benyamini, Y. (1997). Self-rated health and mortality: A review of twenty-seven community studies. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 38, 21–37.  https://doi.org/10.2307/2955359 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Ivey, K., Allen, R. S., Liu, Y., Parmelee, P. A., & Zarit, S. H. (2018). Immediate and lagged effects of daily stress and affect on caregivers’ daily pain experience. The Gerontologist, 5, 913–922.  https://doi.org/10.1093/geront/gnx099 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Jakobsson, U., Hallberg, I. R., & Westergren, A. (2004). Overall and health related quality of life among the oldest old in pain. Quality of Life Research, 13, 125–136.  https://doi.org/10.1023/B:QURE.0000015286.68287.66 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Jensen, M. P., Karoly, P., & Braver, S. (1986). The measurement of clinical pain intensity: A comparison of six methods. Pain, 27, 117–126.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0304-3959(86)90228-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Jones, D. R., Johnson, J. A., Graham-Engeland, J. E., Park, C. L., & Smyth, J. M. (2018). Is perceived growth associated with momentary indicators of health and well-being in people with asthma or rheumatoid arthritis? Applied Psychology: Health and Well-Being, 10, 254–271.  https://doi.org/10.1111/aphw.12129 Google Scholar
  43. Keefe, J., Lumley, M., Anderson, T., Lynch, T., & Carson, K. L. (2001). Pain and emotion: New research directions. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 57, 587–607.  https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.1030 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Kothari, D. J., Davis, M. C., Yeung, E. W., & Tennen, H. A. (2015). Positive affect and pain: Mediators of the within-day relation linking sleep quality to activity interference in fibromyalgia. Pain, 156, 540–546.  https://doi.org/10.1097/01.j.pain.0000460324.18138.0a CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Koval, P., Sütterlin, S., & Kuppens, P. (2016). Emotional inertia is associated with lower well-being when controlling for differences in emotional context. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1–11.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01997 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Kratz, A. L., Mary, M. A., Davis, C., & Zautra, A. J. (2007). Pain acceptance moderates the relation between pain and negative affect in female osteoarthritis and fibromyalgia patients. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 33, 291–301.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02879911 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Kroenke, K., Wu, J., Bair, M. J., Krebs, E. E., Damush, T. M., & Tu, W. (2011). Reciprocal relationship between pain and depression: A 12-month longitudinal analysis in primary care. The Journal of Pain, 12, 964–973.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2011.03.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Kwissa-Gajewska, Z. K., & Gruszczynska, E. (2018). Relationship between daily pain and affect in women with rheumatoid arthritis: Lower optimism as a vulnerability factor. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 41, 12–21.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10865-017-9874-7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Labouvie-Vief, G. (2003). Dynamic integration: Affect, cognition and the self in adulthood. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 201–206.  https://doi.org/10.1046/j.0963-7214.2003.01262.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Larsen, J. T. (2017). Holes in the case for mixed emotions. Emotion Review, 9, 118–123.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073916639662 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Leeuw, M., Goossens, M. E. J. B., Linton, S. J., Crombez, G., Boersma, K., & Vlaeyen, J. W. S. (2007). The fear-avoidance model of musculoskeletal pain: Current state of scientific evidence. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 30, 77–94.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10865-006-9085-0 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Loebach Wetherell, J., Afari, N., Rutledge, T., Sorrell, J. T., Stoddard, J. A., Petkus, A. J., et al. (2011). A randomized, controlled trial of acceptance and commitment therapy and cognitive-behavioral therapy for chronic pain. Pain, 152, 2098–2107.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2011.05.016 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Lumley, M. A., Cohen, J. L., Borszcz, G. S., Cano, A., Radcliffe, A. M., Porter, L. S., et al. (2011). Pain and emotion: A biopsychosocial review of recent research. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 67, 942–968.  https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.20816 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Martin, M., Jäncke, L., & Röcke, C. (2012). Functional approaches to lifespan development: Toward aging research as the science of stabilization. GeroPsych, 25, 185–188.  https://doi.org/10.1024/1662-9647/a000069 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. McCracken, L. M., & Vowles, K. E. (2014). Acceptance and commitment therapy and mindfulness for chronic pain: Model, process, and progress. American Psychologist, 69, 178–187.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035623 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Molenaar, P. C. M., & Campbell, C. G. (2009). The new person-specific paradigm in psychology. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 2, 112–117.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01619.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Mroczek, D. K., & Kolarz, C. M. (1998). The effect of age on positive and negative affect: A developmental perspective on happiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1333–1349.  https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.75.5.1333 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Nesselroade, J. R., & Schmidt McCollam, K. M. (2000). Putting the process in developmental processes. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 24(295), 300.  https://doi.org/10.1080/01650250050118277 Google Scholar
  59. Newth, S., & Delongis, A. (2004). Individual differences, mood, and coping with Chronic pain in rheumatoid arthritis: A daily process analysis. Psychology and Health, 19, 283–305.  https://doi.org/10.1080/0887044042000193451 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Niermann, C. Y. N., Herrmann, C., von Haaren, B., van Kann, D., & Woll, A. (2016). Affect and subsequent physical activity: An ambulatory assessment study examining the affect-activity association in a real-life context. Frontiers in Psychology.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00677 Google Scholar
  61. Novy, D. M., Nelson, D. V., Hetzel, R. D., Squitieri, P., & Kennington, M. (1998). Coping with chronic pain: Sources of intrinsic and contextual variability. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 21, 19–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Ong, A. D. (2010). Pathways linking positive emotion and health in later life. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19, 358–362.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721410388805 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Ong, A. D., Zautra, A. J., & Reid, M. C. (2010). Psychological resilience predicts decreases in pain catastrophizing through positive emotions. Psychology and Aging, 25, 516–523.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019384 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Pandey, R., & Choubey, A. K. (2010). Emotion and health: An overview. Journal of Projective Psychology and Mental Health, 17, 135–152.Google Scholar
  65. Park, D. C., Hertzog, C., Leventhal, H., Morrell, R. W., Leventhal, E., Birchmore, D., et al. (1999). Medication adherence in rheumatoid arthritis patients: Older is wiser. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 47, 172–183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Patel, K. V., Guralnik, J. M., Dansie, E. J., & Turk, D. C. (2013). Prevalence and impact of pain among older adults in the United States: Findings from the 2011 national health and aging trends study. Pain, 154, 2649–2657.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2013.07.029 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Peugh, J. L. (2010). A practical guide to multilevel modeling. Journal of School Psychology, 48, 85–112.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2009.09.002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Pinheiro, J., Bates, D., DebRoy, S., Sarkar, D., & R Core Team. (2019). nlme: Linear and nonlinear mixed effects models. R package version 3.1-141. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=nlme
  69. Pressman, S. D., & Cohen, S. (2005). Does positive affect influence health? Psychological Bulletin, 131, 925–971.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.131.6.925 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Pressman, S. D., Jenkins, B. N., Kraft-Feil, T. L., Rasmussen, H., & Scheier, M. F. (2017). The whole is not the sum of its parts: Specific types of positive affect influence sleep differentially. Emotion, 17(5), 778–793.  https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000256 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. R Core Team. (2017). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria. Retrieved from http://www.r-project.org/
  72. Ramirez-Maestre, C., Lopez Martinez, A., & Zarazaga, R. E. (2004). Personality characteristics as differential variables of the pain experience. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 5, 59.Google Scholar
  73. Roberts, B. W., & Mroczek, D. (2008). Personality trait change in adulthood. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 31–35.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00543.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Röcke, C., & Brose, A. (2013). Intraindividual variability and stability of affect and well-being. GeroPsych, 26, 185–199.  https://doi.org/10.1024/1662-9647/a000094 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Röcke, C., Li, S., & Smith, J. (2009). Intraindividual variability in positive and negative affect over 45 days: Do older adults fluctuate less than young adults? Psychology and Aging, 24, 863–878.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016276 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 1161–1178.  https://doi.org/10.1037/h0077714 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Salthouse, T. A. (2009). When does age-related cognitive decline begin? Neurobiology of Aging, 30, 507–514.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.09.023 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Sandstrom, G. M., Lathia, N., Mascolo, C., & Rentfrow, P. J. (2017). Putting mood in context: Using smartphones to examine how people feel in different locations. Journal of Research in Personality, 69, 96–101.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2016.06.004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Schwerdtfeger, A. R., Friedrich-Mai, P., Kathrin, A., & Gerteis, S. (2015). Daily positive affect and nocturnal cardiac activation. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 22, 132–138.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s12529-014-9396-4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Shacham, S., Reinhardt, L. C., Raubertas, R. F., & Cleeland, C. S. (1983). Emotional states and pain: Intraindividual and interindividual measures of association. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 6, 405–419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Simsek, I. E., Simsek, T. T., Yümin, E. T., Sertel, M., & Oztürk, A. (2010). The effects of pain on health-related quality of life and satisfaction with life in older adults. Topics in Geriatric Rehabilitation, 26, 361–367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Sliwinski, M. J. (2008). Measurement-burst designs for social health research. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2, 245–261.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2007.00043.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Song, S., Graham-Engeland, J. E., Mogle, J. A., & Martire, L. M. (2015). The effects of daily mood and couple interactions on the sleep quality of older adults with chronic pain. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 38, 944–955.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10865-015-9651-4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Sorbi, M. J., Peters, M. L., Kruise, D. A., Maas, C. J. M., Kerssens, J. J., Verhaak, P. F. M., et al. (2006). Electronic momentary assessment in chronic pain II: Pain and psychological pain responses as predictors of pain disability. Clinical Journal of Pain, 22, 67–81.  https://doi.org/10.1097/01.ajp.0000148625.84874.48 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Stone, A. A., Schwartz, J. E., Broderick, J. E., & Deaton, A. (2010). A snapshot of the age distribution of psychological well-being in the United States. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107,.  https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1003744107 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  86. Strauss, E., Macdonald, S. W. S., Hunter, M., Moll, A., & Hultsch, D. F. (2002). Intraindividual variability in cognitive performance in three groups of older adults: Cross-domain links to physical status and self-perceived affect and beliefs. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8, 893–906.  https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617702870035 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. Ussher, M., Spatz, A., Copland, C., Nicolaou, A., Cargill, A., Amini-Tabrizi, N., et al. (2014). Immediate effects of a brief mindfulness-based body scan on patients with chronic pain. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 37, 127–134.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10865-012-9466-5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  88. van Damme, S., Crombez, G., & Lorenz, J. (2007). Pain draws visual attention to its location: Experimental evidence for a threat-related bias. The Journal of Pain, 8, 976–982.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2007.07.005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  90. Wickham, R. E., & Knee, C. R. (2013). Examining temporal processes in diary studies. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 39, 1184–1198.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167213490962 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  91. Wilhelm, P., & Schoebi, D. (2007). Assessing mood in daily life. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 23, 258–267.  https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759.23.4.258 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  92. Willman, A., Petzäll, K., Östberg, A. L., & Hall-Lord, M. L. (2013). The psycho-social dimension of pain and health-related quality of life in the oldest old. Scandinavian Journal of Caring Sciences, 27, 534–540.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6712.2012.01062.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  93. Wong, W. S., Lam, H. M. J., Chen, P. P., Chow, Y. F., Wong, S., Lim, H. S., et al. (2015). The fear-avoidance model of chronic pain: Assessing the role of neuroticism and negative affect in pain catastrophizing using structural equation modeling. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 22, 118–131.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s12529-014-9413-7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  94. Zautra, A. J., Johnson, L. M., & Davis, M. C. (2005). Positive affect as a source of resilience for women in chronic pain. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, 212–220.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.73.2.212 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  95. Zautra, A. J., Smith, B., Affleck, G., & Tennen, H. (2001). Examinations of chronic pain and affect relationships: Applications of a dynamic model of affect. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 69, 786–795.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.69.5.786 CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of ZurichZurichSwitzerland
  2. 2.University Research Priority Program “Dynamics of Healthy Aging”University of ZurichZurichSwitzerland

Personalised recommendations