Advertisement

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

, Volume 49, Issue 12, pp 4780–4787 | Cite as

Memory for Rules and Output Monitoring in Adults with Autism Spectrum Disorder

  • Kenta YamamotoEmail author
  • Kouhei Masumoto
Original Paper

Abstract

This study examined factors related to repetitive errors in people with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) from the perspective of output monitoring and memory for rules. Previous studies have suggested that output monitoring errors are associated with repetition errors. Moreover, people with ASD have a reduced memory for rules, which could result in repetitive errors. Typically developing (TD) and ASD participants memorized rules and conducted an object arrangement task consisting of sorting objects according to their price under two conditions. Memory tests and output monitoring tests were conducted immediately, and 1 week later. Results indicated that output monitoring in ASD was significantly lower than in TD, although the memory for rules showed no differences between ASD and TD.

Keywords

Output monitoring Memory of rules Item memory Repetition errors Restricted Object arrangement task 

Notes

Funding

This work was supported by a Grant-in-Aid for JSPS (Japan Society for the Promotion of Science) KAKENHI (Grant Numbers 18J12157, 17KK0049).

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of interest

Author Kenta Yamamoto declares that he has no conflict of interest. Author Kouhei Masumoto declares that he has no conflict of interest.

Supplementary material

10803_2019_4186_MOESM1_ESM.docx (2.9 mb)
Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 2922 kb)

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Barnard, L., Muldoon, K., Hasan, R., O’Brien, G., & Stewart, M. (2008). Profiling executive dysfunction in adults with autism and comorbid learning disability. Autism,12(2), 125–141.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  3. Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001). The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,31(1), 5–17.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bogte, H., Flamma, B., van der Meere, J. D., & van Engeland, H. (2008). Cognitive flexibility in adults with high functioning autism. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology,30(1), 33–41.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  5. Bowler, D. A., Gaigg, S. B., & Gardiner, J. M. (2008). Effects of related and unrelated context on recall and recognition by adults with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia,46(4), 993–999.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bowler, D. M., Gaigg, S. B., & Gardiner, J. M. (2014). Binding of multiple features in memory by high-functioning adults with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,44(9), 2355–2362.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  7. Cohen, R. L. (1989). Memory for action events: The power of enactment. Educational Psychology Review,1(1), 57–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Cyr, A. A., & Anderson, N. D. (2015). Mistakes as stepping stones: Effects of errors on episodic memory among younger and older adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition,41(3), 841–850.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Eichenbaum, H. (2004). Hippocampus: Cognitive processes and neural representations that underlie declarative memory. Neuron,44(1), 109–120.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Eichenbaum, H., Yonelinas, A. P., & Ranganath, C. (2007). The medial temporal lobe and recognition memory. Annual Review of Neuroscience,30, 123–152.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Frith, U., & Happe, F. (1999). Theory of mind and self-consciousness: What is it like to be autistic? Mind & Language,14(1), 1–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Gaigg, S. B., Bowler, D. M., Ecker, C., Calvo-Merino, B., & Murphy, D. G. (2015). Episodic recollection difficulties in ASD result from atypical relational encoding: Behavioral and neural evidence. Autism Research,8(3), 317–327.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Gaigg, S. B., Gardiner, J. M., & Bowler, D. A. (2008). Free recall in autism spectrum disorder: The role of relational and item-specific encoding. Neuropsychologia,46(4), 983–992.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Gardiner, J. M., & Klee, H. (1976). Memory for remembered events-assessment of output monitoring in free-recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior,15(2), 227–233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Gardiner, J. M., Passmore, C., Herriot, P., & Klee, H. (1977). Memory for remembered events—Effects of response mode and response-produced feedback. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior,16(1), 45–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Grisdale, E., Lind, S. E., Eacott, M. J., & Williams, D. M. (2014). Self-referential memory in autism spectrum disorder and typical development: Exploring the ownership effect. Consciousness and Cognition,30, 133–141.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  17. Hamann, S. B., & Squire, L. R. (1995). On the acquisition of new declarative knowledge in amnesia. Behavioral Neuroscience,109(6), 1027–1044.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Hare, D. J., Mellor, C., & Azmi, S. (2007). Episodic memory in adults with autistic spectrum disorders: Recall for self-versus other-experienced events. Research in Developmental Disabilities,28(3), 317–329.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  19. Hofvander, B., Delorme, R., Chaste, P., Nyden, A., Wentz, E., Stahlberg, O., et al. (2009). Psychiatric and psychosocial problems in adults with normal-intelligence autism spectrum disorders. BMC Psychiatry,9, 35.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Klee, H., & Gardiner, J. M. (1976). Memory for remembered events—Contrasting recall and recognition. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior,15(4), 471–478.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Klee, H., & Gardiner, J. M. (1980). Remembering the recall of cued and uncued words—Effects of initial accessibility. Canadian Journal of Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie,34(3), 220–226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Klee, H., & Legge, D. (1980). Remembering the recall of words. Canadian Journal of Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie,34(1), 86–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Konkel, A., Warren, D. E., Duff, M. C., Tranel, D. N., & Cohen, N. J. (2008). Hippocampal amnesia impairs all manner of relational memory. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience,2, 15.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Koriat, A., & Ben-Zur, H. (1988). Remembering that I did it: Processes and deficits in output monitoring. In M. M. Gruneberg, P. E. Morris, & R. N. Sykes (Eds.), Practical aspects of memory: Current research and issues (Vol. 1, pp. 203–208)., Memory in everyday life Oxford, England: Wiley.Google Scholar
  25. Kurita, H., Koyama, T., & Osada, H. (2005). Autism-spectrum quotient-Japanese version and its short forms for screening normally intelligent persons with pervasive developmental disorders. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences,59(4), 490–496.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Landry, R., & Bryson, S. E. (2004). Impaired disengagement of attention in young children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,45(6), 1115–1122.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Leynes, P. A., & Bink, M. L. (2002). Did I do that? An ERP study of memory for performed and planned actions. International Journal of Psychophysiology,45(3), 197–210.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Leynes, P. A., Crawford, J., & Bink, M. (2005). Interrupted actions affect output monitoring and event-related potentials (ERPs). Memory,13(7), 759–772.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Liss, M., Fein, D., Allen, D., Dunn, M., Feinstein, C., Morris, R., et al. (2001). Executive functioning in high-functioning children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines,42(2), 261–270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Lombardo, M. V., Barnes, J. L., Wheelwright, S. J., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2007). Self-referential cognition and empathy in autism. PLoS ONE,2(9), e883.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Maier, S., van Elst, L. T., Beier, D., Ebert, D., Fangmeier, T., Radtke, M., et al. (2015). Increased hippocampal volumes in adults with high functioning autism spectrum disorder and an IQ > 100: A manual morphometric study. Psychiatry Research-Neuroimaging,234(1), 152–155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Marsh, R. L., Hicks, J. L., Hancock, T. W., & Munsayac, K. (2002). Investigating the output monitoring component of event-based prospective memory performance. Memory & Cognition,30(2), 302–311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Mayes, A., Montaldi, D., & Migo, E. (2007). Associative memory and the medial temporal lobes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,11(3), 126–135.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Misic, B., Doesburg, S. M., Fatima, Z., Vidal, J., Vakorin, V. A., Taylor, M. J., et al. (2015). Coordinated information generation and mental flexibility: Large-scale network disruption in children with autism. Cerebral Cortex,25(9), 2815–2827.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Norman, D. A. (1981). Categorization of action slips. Psychological Review,88(1), 1–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Squire, L. R. (1992). Memory and the hippocampus—A synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychological Review,99(2), 195–231.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Stewart, M. E., Barnard, L., Pearson, J., Hasan, R., & O’Brien, G. (2006). Presentation of depression in autism and Asperger syndrome: A review. Autism,10(1), 103–116.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Sugimori, E., & Asai, T. (2015). Attribution of movement: Potential links between subjective reports of agency and output monitoring. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,68(5), 900–916.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Sugimori, E., & Kusumi, T. (2009). Limiting attentional resources influences performance and output monitoring of an event-based prospective memory task. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology,21(1), 112–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Toichi, M., Kamio, Y., Okada, T., Sakihama, M., Youngstrom, E. A., Findling, R. L., et al. (2002). A lack of self-consciousness in autism. American Journal of Psychiatry,159(8), 1422–1424.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Uhlhaas, P. J., & Singer, W. (2007). What do disturbances in neural synchrony tell us about autism? Biological Psychiatry,62(3), 190–191.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Woodbury-Smith, M. R., Robinson, J., Wheelwright, S., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2005). Screening adults for asperger syndrome using the AQ: A preliminary study of its diagnostic validity in clinical practice. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,35(3), 331–335.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Yamamoto, K., & Masumoto, K. (2018). Brief report: Memory for self-performed actions in adults with autism spectrum disorder: Why does memory of self decline in ASD? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,48(9), 3216–3222.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Graduate School of Human DevelopmentKobe UniversityKobeJapan

Personalised recommendations