Construction of Past and Future Events in Children and Adolescents with ASD: Role of Self-relatedness and Relevance to Decision-Making

  • Elisa Ciaramelli
  • Silvia Spoglianti
  • Elena Bertossi
  • Nadia Generali
  • Francesca Telarucci
  • Raffaella Tancredi
  • Filippo Muratori
  • Roberta Igliozzi
Original Paper

Abstract

We studied episodic memory and future thinking for self-relevant and other-relevant events at different levels of retrieval support, theory of mind, and delay discounting in ASD children and adolescents (ASDs). Compared to typically developing controls, ASDs produced fewer internal (episodic) but a similar number of external (semantic) details while remembering past events, imagining future events, and imagining future events happening to others, indicating a general impairment of event construction. This deficit was driven by group differences under high retrieval support, and therefore unlikely to depend on self-initiated retrieval/construction deficits. ASDs’ event construction impairment related to the severity of ASD symptoms, and to theory of mind deficits. ASDs, however, showed normal delay discounting, highlighting preserved forms of future-based decision-making in ASD.

Keywords

Episodic memory Future thinking Autism spectrum disorder Delay discounting Theory of mind 

Notes

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by grants from the University of Bologna (Ricerca Fondamentale Orientata) to E.C., and by a ‘Ricerca Corrente’ (Annual research program) grant from the Italian Ministry of health to the IRCCS - Fondazione Stella Maris. We are grateful to all of participants who took part in this study and their parents. We thank Violetta Knyagnytska for serving as the second independent scorer for participants’ reports.

Author Contributions

EC, RI, SS, and EB conceived the study. All authors contributed to the study design. RI, SS, FT, and NG tested participants. EC and EB analyzed the data. EC wrote the paper. All authors reviewed the manuscript.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

References

  1. Addis, D. R., Wong, A. T., & Schacter, D. L. (2008). Age-related changes in the episodic simulation of future events. Psychological Science, 19, 33–41.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Adler, N., Nadler, B., Eviatar, Z., & Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. (2010). The relationship between theory of mind and autobiographical memory in high-functioning autism and Asperger syndrome. Psychiatry Research, 178, 214–216.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, fourth edition, text revision (DSM-IV-TR). Washington, D.C.: Author.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Antrop, I., Stock, P., Verté, S., Wiersema, J. R., Baeyens, D., & Roeyers, H. (2006). ADHD and delay aversion: The influence of non-temporal stimulation on choice for delayed rewards. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(11), 1152–1158.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a theory of mind? Cognition, 21(1), 37–46.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Baron-Cohen, S., O’Riordan, M., Stone, V., Jones, R., & Plaisted, K. (1999). Recognition of faux pas by normally developing children and children with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(5), 407–418.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Ben Shalom, D. (2003). Memory in autism: Review and synthesis. Cortex, 39, 1129–1138.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Ben Shalom, D. (2009). The medial prefrontal cortex and integration in autism. The Neuroscientist, 15(6), 589–598.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Bertossi, E., Candela, V., De Luca, F., & Ciaramelli, E. (2017). Episodic future thinking following vmPFC damage: Impaired event construction, maintenance, or narration? Neuropsychology, 31(3), 337–348.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Biederman, I. (1981). Do background depth gradients facilitate object identification? Perception, 10(5), 573–578.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Bisiacchi, P. S., Cendron, M., Gugliotta, M., Tressoldi, P. E., & Vio, C. (2005). BVN 5–11—Batteria di valutazione neuropsicologica per l’età evolutiva. Trento: Erickson.Google Scholar
  13. Bowler, D. M., Gaigg, S. B., & Gardiner, J. M. (2014). Binding of multiple features in memory by high-functioning adults with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(9), 2355–2362.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Bowler, D. M., Gardiner, J. M., & Gaigg, S. B. (2007). Factors affecting conscious awareness in the recollective experience of adults with Asperger’s syndrome. Consciousness and Cognition, 16(1), 124–143.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Bowler, D. M., Gardiner, J. M., & Grice, S. J. (2000). Episodic memory and remembering in adults with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(4), 295–304.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Broyd, S. J., Demanuele, C., Debener, S., Helps, S. K., James, C. J., & Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2009). Default-mode brain dysfunction in mental disorders: A systematic review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 33(3), 279–296.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., & Schacter, D. L. (2008). The brain’s default network. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124(1), 1–38.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Buckner, R. L., & Carroll, D. C. (2007). Self-projection and the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(2), 49–57.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Conway, M. A., & Pleydell-Pearce, C. W. (2000). The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system. Psychological Review, 107, 261–288.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Cooper, J. M., Vargha-Khadem, F., Gadian, D. G., & Maguire, E. A. (2011). The effect of hippocampal damage in children on recalling the past and imagining new experiences. Neuropsychologia, 49(7), 1843–1850.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  21. Crane, L., Lind, S. E., & Bowler, D. M. (2013). Remembering the past and imagining the future in autism spectrum disorder. Memory, 21(2), 157–166.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Demurie, E., Roeyers, H., Baeyens, D., & Sonuga-Barke, E. (2012). Temporal discounting of monetary rewards in children and adolescents with ADHD and autism spectrum disorders. Developmental Science, 15(6), 791–800.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Frith, U. (1989). Explaining the enigma. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
  24. Gaesser, B., Sacchetti, D. C., Addis, D. R., & Schacter, D. L. (2011). Characterizing age-related changes in remembering the past and imagining the future. Psychology and Aging, 26(1), 80–84.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  25. Gaigg, S. B., Bowler, D. M., Ecker, C., Calvo-Merino, B., & Murphy, D. G. (2010, May). fMRI correlates of relational memory difficulties in autism spectrum disorder. Poster presented at the International Meeting for Autism Research, Philadelphia.Google Scholar
  26. Goddard, L., Howlin, P., Dritschel, B., & Patel, T. (2007). Autobiographical memory and social problem-solving in Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(2), 291–300.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Green, L., & Myerson, J. (2004). A discounting framework for choice with delayed and probabilistic rewards. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 769–792.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  28. Gugliotta, M., Bisiacchi, P. S., Cendron, M., Tressoldi, P. E., & Vio, C. (2009). BVN 12–18—Batteria di Valutazione Neuropsicologica per l’adolescenza. Trento: Erickson.Google Scholar
  29. Hanson, L. K., & Atance, C. M. (2014). Brief report: Episodic foresight in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44, 674–684.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Happé, F. G. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story characters’ thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(2), 129–154.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Happé, F. G. E., & Booth, R. D. L. (2008). The power of the positive: Revisiting weak coherence in autism spectrum disorders. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006), 61(1), 50–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Hassabis, D., Kumaran, D., & Maguire, E. A. (2007). Using imagination to understand the neural basis of episodic memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(52), 14365–14374.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  33. Hassabis, D., & Maguire, E. A. (2007). Deconstructing episodic memory with construction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(7), 299–306.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Hassabis, D., & Maguire, E. A. (2009). The construction system of the brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1521), 1263–1271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Henderson, J. M., & Hollingworth, A. (1999). High-level scene perception. Annual Review of Psychology, 50(1), 243–271.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Jackson, L. K., & Atance, C. M. (2008). Future thinking in children with autism spectrum disorder: A pilot study. Journal on Developmental Disabilities, 14, 40–45.Google Scholar
  37. Johnson, M. K., Foley, M. A., Suengas, A. G., & Raye, C. L. (1988). Phenomenal characteristics of memories for perceived and imagined autobiographical events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117(4), 371–376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Kable, J. W., & Glimcher, P. W. (2010). An “as soon as possible” effect in human intertemporal decision making: Behavioral evidence and neural mechanisms. Journal of Neurophysiology, 103(5), 2513–2531.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  39. Kaldy, Z., Giserman, I., Carter, A. S., & Blaser, E. (2016). The mechanisms underlying the ASD advantage in visual search. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(5), 1513–1527.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  40. Kennedy, D. P., Redcay, E., & Courchesne, E. (2006). Failing to deactivate: Resting functional abnormalities in autism. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A., 103, 8275–8280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Kenworthy, L., Yerys, B. E., Anthony, L. G., & Wallace, G. L. (2008). Understanding executive control in autism spectrum disorders in the lab and in the real world. Neuropsychology Review, 18(4), 320–338.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  42. Korkman, M., Kirk, U., & Kemp, S. (2007). NEPSY-II: A developmental neuropsychological assessment. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  43. Kwan, D., Craver, C. F., Green, L., Myerson, J., & Rosenbaum, R. S. (2013). Dissociations in future thinking following hippocampal damage: Evidence from discounting and time perspective in episodic amnesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(4), 1355–1369.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Lai, C. L. E., Lau, Z., Lui, S. S. Y., Lok, E., Tam, V., Chan, Q., Cheng, K. M., Lam, S. M., & Cheung, E. F. C. (2017). Meta-analysis of neuropsychological measures of executive functioning in children and adolescents with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research: Official Journal of the International Society for Autism Research, 10(5), 911–939.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretence and representation: The origins of “theory of mind”. Psychological Review, 94, 412–426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Levine, B., Svoboda, E., Hay, J. F., Winocur, G., & Moscovitch, M. (2002). Aging and autobiographical memory: Dissociating episodic from semantic retrieval. Psychology and Aging, 17(4), 677–689.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Lind, S. E. (2010). Memory and the self in autism. Autism, 14(5), 430–456.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Lind, S. E., & Bowler, D. M. (2009). Delayed self-recognition in children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(4), 643–650.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Lind, S. E., & Bowler, D. M. (2010). Episodic memory and episodic future thinking in adults with autism. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119(4), 896–905.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Lind, S. E., Bowler, D. M., & Raber, J. (2014a). Spatial navigation, episodic memory, episodic future thinking, and theory of mind in children with autism spectrum disorder: Evidence for impairments in mental simulation? Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1411.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  51. Lind, S. E., Williams, D. M., Bowler, D. M., & Peel, A. (2014b). Episodic memory and episodic future thinking impairments in high-functioning autism spectrum disorder: An underlying difficulty with scene construction or self-projection? Neuropsychology, 28(1), 55–67.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., Risi, S., Gotham, K., & Bishop, S. L. (2012). ADOS. Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, second edition (ADOS-2). Torrance, CA: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
  53. Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(5), 659–685.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Marini, A., Ferretti, F., Chiera, A., Magni, R., Adornetti, I., Nicchiarelli, S., Vicari, S., & Valeri, G. (2016). Brief report: Self-based and mechanical-based future thinking in children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(10), 3353–3360.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. McCormick, C., Rosenthal, C. R., Miller, T. D., & Maguire, E. A. (2017). Deciding what is possible and impossible following hippocampal damage in humans. Hippocampus, 27(3), 303–314.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  56. McGraw, K. O., & Wong, S. P. (1996). Forming inferences about some intraclass correlation coefficients. Psychological Methods, 1, 30–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Minshew, N. J., & Williams, D. L. (2007). The new neurobiology of autism: Cortex, connectivity, and neuronal organization. Archives of Neurology, 64, 945–950.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  58. Mitchell, P., & O’Keefe, K. (2008). Brief report: Do individuals with autism spectrum disorder think they know their own minds? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(8), 1591–1597.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  59. Moscovitch, M., & Winocur, G. (1992). The neuropsychology of memory and aging. The handbook of aging and cognition (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  60. Moscovitch, M., Yaschyshyn, T., Ziegler, M., & Nadel, L. (1999). Remote episodic memory and retrograde amnesia: Was Endel Tulving right all along? In E. Tulving (Ed.), Memory, consciousness, and the brain: The Tallinn conference (pp. 331–345). Philadelphia: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  61. Mottron, L., Dawson, M., Soulières, I., Hubert, B., & Burack, J. (2006). Enhanced perceptual functioning in Autism: An update, and eight principles of autistic perception. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 27–43.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  62. Myerson, J., Green, L., Hanson, J. S., Holt, D. D., & Estle, S. J. (2003). Discounting delayed and probabilistic rewards: Processes and traits. Journal of Economic Psychology, 24, 619–635.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Myerson, J., Green, L., & Warusawitharana, M. (2001). Area under the curve as a measure of discounting. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 76, 235–243.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  64. Parsons, L., Cordier, R., Munro, N., Joosten, A., & Speyer, R. (2017). A systematic review of pragmatic language interventions for children with autism spectrum disorder. PLoS ONE, 12(4), e0172242.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  65. Pellicano, E., Smith, A. D., Cristino, F., Hood, B. M., Briscoe, J., & Gilchrist, I. D. (2011). Children with autism are neither systematic nor optimal foragers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A., 108, 421–426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Peters, J., & Büchel, C. (2010). Episodic future thinking reduces reward delay discounting through an enhancement of prefrontal-mediotemporal interactions. Neuron, 66(1), 138–148.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  67. Rosenbaum, R. S., Moscovitch, M., Foster, J. K., Schnyer, D. M., Gao, F., Kovacevic, N., Verfaellie, M., Black, S. E., & Levine, B. (2008). Patterns of autobiographical memory loss in medial-temporal lobe amnesic patients. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(8), 1490–1506.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  68. Salmond, C. H., Ashburner, J., Connelly, A., Friston, K. J., Gadian, D. G., & Vargha-Khadem, F. (2005). The role of the medial temporal lobe in autistic spectrum disorders. European Journal of Neuroscience, 22, 764–772.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  69. Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., Hassabis, D., Martin, V. C., Spreng, R. N., & Szpunar, K. K. (2012). The future of memory: Remembering, imagining, and the brain. Neuron, 76(4), 677–694.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  70. Sellitto, M., Ciaramelli, E., Mattioli, F., & di Pellegrino, G. (2016). Reduced sensitivity to sooner reward during intertemporal decision-making following insula damage in humans. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 9, 367.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  71. Sodian, B., & Frith, U. (1992). Deception and sabotage in autistic, retarded and normal children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 33(3), 591–605.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  72. Spreng, R. N., & Grady, C. L. (2010). Patterns of brain activity supporting autobiographical memory, prospection, and theory of mind, and their relationship to the Default Mode Network. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(6), 1112–1123.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  73. Stanfield, A. C., McIntosh, A. M., Spencer, M. D., Philip, R., Gaur, S., & Lawrie, S. M. (2008). Towards a neuroanatomy of autism: A systematic review and meta-analysis of structural magnetic resonance imaging studies. European Psychiatry, 23(4), 289–299.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  74. Suddendorf, T., & Corballis, M. C. (2007). The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans? The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30(3), 299-313-51.Google Scholar
  75. Summerfield, J. J., Hassabis, D., & Maguire, E. A. (2010). Differential engagement of brain regions within a “core” network during scene construction. Neuropsychologia, 48(5), 1501–1509.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  76. Svoboda, E., Hynes, C. A., Campbell, A. F., Dade, L. A., Moscovitch, M., & Levine, B. (2002). The frontal lobes and autobiographical memory: Differential effects of dorsolateral and ventrolateral prefrontal damage. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8, 275.Google Scholar
  77. Terrett, G., Rendell, P. G., Raponi-Saunders, S., Henry, J. D., Bailey, P. E., & Altgassen, M. (2013). Episodic future thinking in children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(11), 2558–2568.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  78. Tressoldi, P. E., Cornoldi, C., & Re, A. M. (2013). BVSCO-2—Batteria per la Valutazione della Scrittura e della Competenza Ortografica-2. Firenze: Giunti O.S. Psychometrics.Google Scholar
  79. Tulving, E. (1985). Memory and consciousness. Canadian Psychology, 26, 1–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Urgesi, C., Campanella, F., & Fabbro, F. (2011). NEPSY-II, Contributo alla Taratura Italiana. Firenze: Giunti OS Psychometrics.Google Scholar
  81. Van der Hallen, R., Evers, K., Brewaeys, K., Van den Noortgate, W., & Wagemans, J. (2015). Global processing takes time: A meta-analysis on local–global visual processing in ASD. Psychological Bulletin, 141(3), 549–573.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  82. Wechsler, D. (1991). Wechsler intelligence scale for children: Third edition manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  83. Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler intelligence scale for children: Fourth edition (WISC-IV). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  84. Willoughby, K. A., Desrocher, M., Levine, B., & Rovet, J. F. (2012). Episodic and semantic autobiographical memory and everyday memory during late childhood and early adolescence. Frontiers in Psychology, 3(FEB), 1–15.Google Scholar
  85. World Health Organization. (1993). The ICD-10 Classification of Mental Disorders and Behavioral Disorders: Clinical descriptions and diagnostic guidelines. Geneva: WHO.Google Scholar
  86. Yirmiya, N., Erel, O., Shaked, M., & Solomonica-Levi, D. (1998). Meta-analyses comparing theory of mind abilities of individuals with autism, individuals with mental retardation, and normally developing individuals. Psychological Bulletin, 124(3), 283–307.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  87. Zeman, A. Z. J., Beschin, N., Dewar, M., & Della Sala, S. (2013). Imagining the present: Amnesia may impair descriptions of the present as well as of the future and the past. Cortex, 49(3), 637–645.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  • Elisa Ciaramelli
    • 1
    • 2
  • Silvia Spoglianti
    • 3
  • Elena Bertossi
    • 1
    • 2
  • Nadia Generali
    • 2
  • Francesca Telarucci
    • 2
  • Raffaella Tancredi
    • 3
  • Filippo Muratori
    • 3
    • 4
  • Roberta Igliozzi
    • 3
  1. 1.Dipartimento di PsicologiaUniversità di BolognaBolognaItaly
  2. 2.Centro di Studi e Ricerche in Neuroscienze CognitiveCesenaItaly
  3. 3.IRCCS Stella Maris FoundationCalambroneItaly
  4. 4.Dipartimento di Medicina Clinica e SperimentaleUniversità di PisaPisaItaly

Personalised recommendations