Advertisement

Card playing enhances speech perception among aging adults: comparison with aging musicians

  • Leah FostickEmail author
Original Investigation
  • 6 Downloads

Abstract

Speech perception and auditory processing have been shown to be enhanced among aging musicians as compared to non-musicians. In the present study, the aim was to test whether these functions are also enhanced among those who are engaged in a non-musical mentally challenging leisure activity (card playing). Three groups of 23 aging adults, aged 60–80 years, were recruited for the study: Musicians, Card players, and Controls. Participants were matched for age, gender, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III Matrix Reasoning, and Digit Span scores. Their performance was measured using auditory spectral and spatial temporal order judgment tests, and four tasks of speech perception in conditions of: no background noise, background noise of speech frequencies, background noise of white noise, and 60% compressed speech. Musicians were better in auditory and speech perception than the other two groups. Card players were similar to Controls in auditory perception tasks, but were better in the speech perception tasks. Non-musician aging adults may be able to improve their speech perception ability by engaging in leisure activity requiring cognitive effort.

Keywords

Music Leisure activity Auditory processing Speech perception 

Notes

Acknowledgments

The author would like to thank Shira Chana Bienstock for her thorough review of this manuscript.

Funding

This research did not receive any specific Grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

References

  1. Aartsen MJ, Smits CHM, van Tilburg T, Knipscheer KCPM, Deeg DJH (2002) Activity in older adults: cause or consequence of cognitive functioning? A longitudinal study on everyday activities and cognitive performance in older adults. J Gerontol Psychol Sci 57:153–162Google Scholar
  2. Alain C, Bernstein LJ (2008) From sounds to meaning: the role of attention during auditory scene analysis. Curr Opin Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg 16(5):485–489Google Scholar
  3. Alain C, McDonald KL (2007) Age-related differences in neuromagnetic brain activity underlying concurrent sound perception. J Neurosci 27(6):1308–1314Google Scholar
  4. Amer T, Kalender B, Hasher L, Trehub SE, Wong Y (2013) Do older professional musicians have cognitive advantages? PLoS ONE 8(8):e71630Google Scholar
  5. Babkoff H, Fostick L (2017) Age-related changes in auditory processing and speech perception: cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses. Eur J Ageing 14(3):269–281Google Scholar
  6. Ball K, Berch DB, Helmers KF, Jobe JB, Leveck MD, Marsiske M, Morris JN, Rebok GW, Smith DM, Tennstedt SL, Unverzagt FW, Willis SL (2002) Effects of cognitive training interventions with older adults: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA 288(18):2271–2281Google Scholar
  7. Ball K, Edwards JD, Ross LA (2007) The impact of speed of processing training on cognitive and everyday functions. J Gerontol Ser B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 62(1):19–31Google Scholar
  8. Barnes LL, De Leon CM, Wilson RS, Bienias JL, Evans DA (2004) Social resources and cognitive decline in a population of older African Americans and whites. Neurology 63(12):2322–2326Google Scholar
  9. Bassuk SS, Glass TA, Berkman LF (1999) Social disengagement and incident cognitive decline in community-dwelling elderly persons. Ann Intern Med 131(3):165–173Google Scholar
  10. Ben-David BM, Chambers CG, Daneman M, Pichora-Fuller MK, Reingold EM, Schneider BA (2011) Effects of aging and noise on real-time spoken word recognition: evidence from eye movements. J Speech Lang Hear Res 54(1):243–262Google Scholar
  11. Ben-David BM, Vania YY, Schneider BA (2012) Does it take older adults longer than younger adults to perceptually segregate a speech target from a background masker? Hear Res 290(1):55–63Google Scholar
  12. Ben-David BM, Avivi-Reich M, Schneider BA (2016) Does the degree of linguistic experience (native versus nonnative) modulate the degree to which listeners can benefit from a delay between the onset of the maskers and the onset of the target speech? Hear Res 341:9–18Google Scholar
  13. Bidelman GM, Alain C (2015) Musical training orchestrates coordinated neuroplasticity in auditory brainstem and cortex to counteract age-related declines in categorical vowel perception. J Neurosci 35(3):1240–1249Google Scholar
  14. Bidelman GM, Krishnan A (2010) Effects of reverberation on brainstem representation of speech in musicians and non-musicians. Brain Res 1355:112–125Google Scholar
  15. Boothroyd A (1968) Statistical theory of the speech discrimination score. J Acoust Soc Am 43(2):362–367Google Scholar
  16. Bosma H, van Boxtel MP, Ponds RW, Jelicic M, Houx P, Metsemakers J et al (2002) Engaged lifestyle and cognitive function in middle and old-aged, non-demented persons: a reciprocal association? Z fu¨r Gerontol und Geriatr 35:575–581Google Scholar
  17. Carlyon RP (2004) How the brain separates sounds. Trends Cogn Sci 8(10):465–471Google Scholar
  18. Cohen S (2004) Social relationships and health. Am Psychol 59(8):676–684Google Scholar
  19. Coyle JT (2003) Use it or lose it—do effortful mental activities protect against dementia? N Engl J Med 348(25):2489–2490Google Scholar
  20. Dartigues JF, Foubert-Samier A, Le Goff M, Viltard M, Amieva H, Orgogozo JM, Helmer C (2013) Playing board games, cognitive decline and dementia: a French population-based cohort study. BMJ Open 3(8):e002998Google Scholar
  21. Ezzatian P, Li L, Pichora-Fuller K, Schneider BA (2015) Delayed stream segregation in older adults: more than just informational masking. Ear Hear 36(4):482–484Google Scholar
  22. Fitzgibbons PJ, Gordon-Salant S (2010) Behavioral Studies With Aging Humans: Hearing Sensitivity and Psychoacoustics. In: Gordon-Salant S, Frisina RD, Popper AN (eds) The Aging Auditory System. Springer, New York, pp 111–134Google Scholar
  23. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR (1975) Mini-Mental State: a practical method for grading the state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 12:189–198Google Scholar
  24. Fostick L, Babkoff H (2013) Temporal and non-temporal processes in the elderly. J Basic Clin Physiol Pharmacol 24(3):191–199Google Scholar
  25. Fostick L, Ben-Artzi E, Babkoff H (2013) Aging and speech perception: beyond hearing threshold and cognitive ability. J Basic Clin Physiol Pharmacol 24(3):175–183Google Scholar
  26. Fostick L, Babkoff H, Zukerman G (2014) Effect of 24 h of sleep deprivation on auditory and linguistic perception: a comparison among young controls, sleep deprived participants, dyslexic readers, and aging adults. J Speech Lang Hear Res 57:1078–1088Google Scholar
  27. François C, Chobert J, Besson M, Schön D (2013) Music training for the development of speech segmentation. Cereb Cortex 23(9):2038–2043Google Scholar
  28. Getzmann S, Näätänen R (2015) The mismatch negativity as a measure of auditory stream segregation in a simulated “cocktail-party” scenario: effect of age. Neurobiol Aging 36(11):3029–3037Google Scholar
  29. Gow AJ, Avlund K, Mortensen EL (2014) Leisure activity associated with cognitive ability level, but not cognitive change. Front Psychol 5:1176Google Scholar
  30. Heinrich A, Schneider BA (2011) Elucidating the effects of ageing on remembering perceptually distorted word pairs. Q J Exp Psychol 64(1):186–205Google Scholar
  31. Heinrich A, Henshaw H, Ferguson MA (2015) The relationship of speech intelligibility with hearing sensitivity, cognition, and perceived hearing difficulties varies for different speech perception tests. Front Psychol 6:782Google Scholar
  32. Heinrich A, Gagne JP, Viljanen A, Levy DA, Ben-David B, Schneider BA (2016) Effective communication as a fundamental aspect of active aging and well-being: paying attention to the challenges older adults face in noisy environments. Soc Inq Well-Being 2(1):51–69Google Scholar
  33. Herholz SC, Zatorre RJ (2012) Musical training as a framework for brain plasticity: behavior, function, and structure. Neuron 76(3):486–502Google Scholar
  34. Hertzog C, Kramer AF, Wilson RS, Lindenberger U (2008) Enrichment effects on adult cognitive development: can the functional capacity of older adults be preserved and enhanced? Psychol Sci Public Interest 9(1):1–65Google Scholar
  35. Hultsch DF, Small BJ, Hertzog C, Dixon RA (1999) Use it or lose it: engaged lifestyle as a buffer of cognitive decline in aging. Psychol Aging 14:245–263Google Scholar
  36. Humes LE (2007) The contributions of audibility and cognitive factors to the benefit provided by amplified speech to older adults. J Am Acad Audiol 18(7):590–603Google Scholar
  37. Kraus N, Slater J, Thompson EC, Hornickel J, Strait DL, Nicol T, White Schwoch T (2014) Music enrichment programs improve the neural encoding of speech in at-risk children. J Neurosci 34(36):11913–11918Google Scholar
  38. Musacchia G, Sams M, Skoe E, Kraus N (2007) Musicians have enhanced subcortical auditory and audiovisual processing of speech and music. Proc Natl Acad Sci 104(40):15894–15898Google Scholar
  39. O’Brien JL, Nikjeh DA, Lister JJ (2015) Interaction of Musicianship and Aging: a comparison of cortical auditory evoked potentials. Behav Neurol 2015:545917Google Scholar
  40. Parbery-Clark A, Skoe E, Kraus N (2009a) Musical experience limits the degradative effects of background noise on the neural processing of sound. Journal of Neuroscience 29(45):14100–14107Google Scholar
  41. Parbery-Clark A, Skoe E, Lam C, Kraus N (2009b) Musician enhancement for speech-in-noise. Ear Hear 30(6):653–661Google Scholar
  42. Parbery-Clark A, Strait DL, Anderson S, Hittner E, Kraus N (2011) Musical experience and the aging auditory system: implications for cognitive abilities and hearing speech in noise. PLoS ONE 6(5):e18082Google Scholar
  43. Parbery-Clark A, Anderson S, Hittner E, Kraus N (2012) Musical experience offsets age-related delays in neural timing. Neurobiol Aging 33(7):1483-e1Google Scholar
  44. Park DC, Lodi-Smith J, Drew L, Haber S, Hebrank A, Bischof GN, Aamodt W (2014) The impact of sustained engagement on cognitive function in older adults: the synapse project. Psychol Sci 25(1):103–112Google Scholar
  45. Pichora-Fuller MK, Souza PE (2003) Effects of aging on auditory processing of speech. Int J Audiol 42(sup2):11–16Google Scholar
  46. Rammsayer T, Altenmüller E (2006) Temporal information processing in musicians and nonmusicians. Music Percept Interdiscip J 24(1):37–48Google Scholar
  47. Schooler C, Mulatu MS (2001) The reciprocal effects of leisure time activities and intellectual functioning in older people: a longitudinal analysis. Psychol Aging 16:466–482Google Scholar
  48. Snyder JS, Alain C (2005) Age-related changes in neural activity associated with concurrent vowel segregation. Cogn Brain Res 24(3):492–499Google Scholar
  49. Souza PE, Boike KT, Witherell K, Tremblay K (2007) Prediction of speech recognition from audibility in older listeners with hearing loss: effects of age, amplification, and background noise. J Am Acad Audiol 18(1):54–65Google Scholar
  50. Stern C, Munn Z (2010) Cognitive leisure activities and their role in preventing dementia: a systematic review. Int J Evidence-Based Healthc 8(1):2–17Google Scholar
  51. Strait DL, Kraus N, Skoe E, Ashly R (2009) Musical experience promotes subcortical efficiency in processing emotional vocal sounds. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1169(1):209–213Google Scholar
  52. Taitelbaum-Swead R, Fostick L (2016) the effect of age and type of noise on speech perception under conditions of changing context and noise levels. Folia Phoniatr et Logop 68(1):16–21Google Scholar
  53. Verghese J, Lipton RB, Katz MJ, Hall CB, Derby CA, Kuslansky G, Ambrose AF, Sliwinski M, Buschke H (2003) Leisure activities and the risk of dementia in the elderly. N Engl J Med 348:2508–2516Google Scholar
  54. Vinkers DJ, Gussekloo J, Westendorp RGJ (2003) Leisure activities and the risk of dementia. N Engl J Med 349(13):1290–1291Google Scholar
  55. Wang JYJ, Zhou DHD, Li J, Zhang M, Deng J, Tang M, Chen M (2006) Leisure activity and risk of cognitive impairment: the Chongqing aging study. Neurology 66(6):911–913Google Scholar
  56. Wechsler D (1997) WAIS-III: Wechsler adult intelligence scale. Psychol Corporation, San AntonioGoogle Scholar
  57. White-Schwoch T, Carr KW, Anderson S, Strait DL, Kraus N (2013) Older adults benefit from music training early in life: biological evidence for long-term training-driven plasticity. J Neurosci 33(45):17667–17674Google Scholar
  58. Willis SL, Tennstedt SL, Marsiske M, Ball K, Elias J, Koepke KM, Morris JN, Rebok GW, Unverzaqt FW, Stoddard AM, Wright E (2006) Long-term effects of cognitive training on everyday functional outcomes in older adults. JAMA 296(23):2805–2814Google Scholar
  59. Wilson RS, Mendes de Leon CF, Barnes LL, Schneider JA, Bienias JL, Evans DA et al (2002) Participation in cognitively stimulating activities and risk of incident Alzheimer’s disease. J Am Med Assoc 287:742–748Google Scholar
  60. Wilson RS, Bennett DA, Bienias JL, Mendes de Leon CF, Morris MC, Evans DA (2003) Cognitive activity and cognitive decline in a biracial community population. Neurology 61:812–816Google Scholar
  61. Wilson RS, Scherr PA, Schneider JA, Li Y, Bennett DA (2007) The relation of cognitive activity to risk of developing Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 69:1911–1920Google Scholar
  62. Zendel BR, Alain C (2013) The influence of lifelong musicianship on neurophysiological measures of concurrent sound segregation. J Cogn Neurosci 25(4):503–516Google Scholar
  63. Zunzunegui MV, Alvarado BE, Del Ser T, Otero A (2003) Social networks, social integration, and social engagement determine cognitive decline in community-dwelling Spanish older adults. J Gerontol Ser B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 58(2):S93–S100Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature B.V. 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Communication DisordersAriel UniversityArielIsrael

Personalised recommendations