Advertisement

Where did that noise come from? Memory for sound locations is exceedingly eccentric both in front and in rear space

  • Franco DeloguEmail author
  • Phillip McMurray
Research Article
  • 5 Downloads

Abstract

Few studies have examined the stability of the representation of the position of sound sources in spatial working memory. The goal of this study was to verify whether the memory of sound position declines as maintenance time increases. In two experiments, we tested the influence of the delay between stimulus and response in a sound localization task. In Experiment 1, blindfolded participants listened to bursts of white noise originating from 16 loudspeakers equally spaced in a 360-degree circular space around the listener in such a way that the nose was aligned to the zero-degree coordinate. Their task was to indicate sounds’ position using a digital pointer when prompted at varying delays: 0, 3, and 6 s after stimulus offset. In Experiment 2, the task was analogous to Exp. 1 with stimulus–response delays of 0 or 10 s. Results of the two experiments show that increasing stimulus–response delays up to 10 s do not impair sound localization. Participants systematically overestimated the eccentricity of the auditory stimulus by shifting their responses either toward the 90-degree coordinate, in alignment with the right ear, or toward the 270-degree coordinate, in alignment with the left ear. Such bias was analogous in the front and in the rear azimuthal space and was only marginally influenced by the delay conditions. We conclude that the representation of auditory space in working memory is stable, but directionally biased with systematic overestimation of eccentricity.

Keywords

Eccentricity Sound localization Oblique effect Spatial working memory Directional bias 

Notes

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that there is no conflict of interest regarding the publication of this article.

References

  1. Alais D, Burr D (2004) The ventriloquist effect results from near-optimal bimodal integration. Curr Biol 14(3):257–262Google Scholar
  2. Appelle S (1972) Perception and discrimination as a function of stimulus orientation: the” oblique effect” in man and animals. Psychol Bull 78(4):266Google Scholar
  3. Aytekin M, Moss CF, Simon JZ (2008) A sensorimotor approach to sound localization. Neural Comput 20(3):603–635Google Scholar
  4. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y (1995) Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J R Stat Soc Ser B Methodol 57(1):289–300Google Scholar
  5. Bertelson P, Radeau M (1981) Cross-modal bias and perceptual fusion with auditory-visual spatial discordance. Percept Psychophys 29(6):578–584Google Scholar
  6. Botta F, Santangelo V, Raffone A, Sanabria D, Lupiáñez J, Belardinelli MO (2011) Multisensory integration affects visuo-spatial working memory. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 37(4):1099Google Scholar
  7. Briley PM, Kitterick PT, Summerfield AQ (2013) Evidence for opponent process analysis of sound source location in humans. J Assoc Res Otolaryngol 14(1):83–101Google Scholar
  8. Brunetti M, Belardinelli P, Caulo M, Del Gratta C, Della Penna S, Ferretti A, Lucci G, Moretti A, Pizzella V, Tartaro A, Torquati K (2005) Human brain activation during passive listening to sounds from different locations: an fMRI and MEG study. Hum Brain Mapp 26(4):251–261Google Scholar
  9. Butler RA (1986) The bandwidth effect on monaural and binaural localization. Hear Res 21(1):67–73Google Scholar
  10. Butler RA, Musicant AD (1993) Binaural localization: influence of stimulus frequency and the linkage to covert peak areas. Hear Res 67(1):220–229Google Scholar
  11. Celebrini S, Thorpe S, Trotter Y, Imbert M (1993) Dynamics of orientation coding in area V1 of the awake primate. Vis Neurosci 10(05):811–825Google Scholar
  12. Clarke S, Adriani M, Bellmann A (1998) Distinct short-term memory systems for sound content and sound localization. NeuroReport 9(15):3433–3437Google Scholar
  13. Colburn HS, Latimer JS (1978) Theory of binaural interaction based on auditory-nerve data. III. Joint dependence on interaural time and amplitude differences in discrimination and detection. J Acoust Soc Am 64(1):95–106Google Scholar
  14. De Valois RL, Yund EW, Hepler N (1982) The orientation and direction selectivity of cells in macaque visual cortex. Vision Res 22(5):531–544Google Scholar
  15. Ferlazzo F, Couyoumdjian A, Padovani T, Belardinelli MO (2002) Head-centred meridian effect on auditory spatial attention orienting. Q J Exp Psychol Sect A 55(3):937–963Google Scholar
  16. Furmanski CS, Engel SA (2000) An oblique effect in human primary visual cortex. Nat Neurosci 3(6):535–536Google Scholar
  17. Garcia SE, Jones PR, Rubin GS, Nardini M (2017) Auditory localisation biases increase with sensory uncertainty. Sci Rep 7:40567Google Scholar
  18. Gilkey RH, Anderson TR (1994) The accuracy of absolute localization judgments for speech stimuli. J Vestib Res Equilib Orientat 5(6):487–497Google Scholar
  19. Gilkey RH, Good MD, Ericson MA, Brinkman J, Stewart JM (1995) A pointing technique for rapidly collecting localization responses in auditory research. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput 27(1):1–11Google Scholar
  20. Good MD, Gilkey RH (1996) Sound localization in noise: the effect of signal-to-noise ratio. J Acoust Soc Am 99(2):1108–1117Google Scholar
  21. Goossens HHLM, Van Opstal AJ (1999) Influence of head position on the spatial representation of acoustic targets. J Neurophysiol 81(6):2720–2736Google Scholar
  22. Haber L, Haber RN, Penningroth S, Novak K, Radgowski H (1993) Comparison of nine methods of indicating the direction to objects: data from blind adults. Perception 22(1):35–47Google Scholar
  23. Harper NS, McAlpine D (2004) Optimal neural population coding of an auditory spatial cue. Nature 430(7000):682Google Scholar
  24. Jeffress LA (1948) A place theory of sound localization. J Comp Physiol Psychol 41(1):35Google Scholar
  25. Kennett S, Taylor-Clarke M, Haggard P (2001) Noninformative vision improves the spatial resolution of touch in humans. Curr Biol 11(15):1188–1191Google Scholar
  26. Knudsen EI, Konishi M (1978) A neural map of auditory space in the owl. Science 200(4343):795–797Google Scholar
  27. Konishi M (2003) Coding of auditory space. Annu Rev Neurosci 26(1):31–55Google Scholar
  28. Leaver AM, Rauschecker JP (2016) Functional topography of human auditory cortex. J Neurosci 36(4):1416–1428Google Scholar
  29. Lechelt EC, Eliuk J, Tanne G (1976) Perceptual orientational asymmetries: a comparison of visual and haptic space. Percept Psychophys 20(6):463–469Google Scholar
  30. Lehnert G, Zimmer HD (2008) Modality and domain specific components in auditory and visual working memory tasks. Cogn Process 9(1):53Google Scholar
  31. Lewald J, Ehrenstein WH (1998) Auditory-visual spatial integration: a new psychophysical approach using laser pointing to acoustic targets. J Acoust Soc Am 104(3):1586–1597Google Scholar
  32. Lewald J, Ehrenstein WH (2001) Spatial coordinates of human auditory working memory. Cogn Brain Res 12(1):153–159Google Scholar
  33. Lewald J, Dörrscheidt GJ, Ehrenstein WH (2000) Sound localization with eccentric head position. Behav Brain Res 108(2):105–125Google Scholar
  34. Makous JC, Middlebrooks JC (1990) Two-dimensional sound localization by human listeners. J Acoust Soc Am 87(5):2188–2200Google Scholar
  35. Matsumoto M, Scripture EW (1897) Researches on acoustic space. Studies from the Yale Psychological Laboratory 5:1–75Google Scholar
  36. McAlpine D (2005) Creating a sense of auditory space. J Physiol 566(1):21–28Google Scholar
  37. McAlpine D, Jiang D, Palmer AR (2001) A neural code for low-frequency sound localization in mammals. Nat Neurosci 4(4):396Google Scholar
  38. McCarthy L, Olsen KN (2017) A “looming bias” in spatial hearing? Effects of acoustic intensity and spectrum on categorical sound source localization. Atten Percept Psychophys 79(1):352–362Google Scholar
  39. Middlebrooks JC (1992) Narrow-band sound localization related to external ear acoustics. J Acoust Soc Am 92(5):2607–2624Google Scholar
  40. Middlebrooks JC, Green DM (1991) Sound localization by human listeners. Annu Rev Psychol 42(1):135–159Google Scholar
  41. Mills AW (1958) On the minimum audible angle. J Acoust Soc Am 30(4):237–246Google Scholar
  42. Molino JA (1974) Psychophysical verification of predicted interaural differences in localizing distant sound sources. J Acoust Soc Am 55(1):139–147Google Scholar
  43. Newton VE (1983) Sound localisation in children with a severe unilateral hearing loss. Audiology 22(2):189–198Google Scholar
  44. Oldfield SR, Parker SP (1984) Acuity of sound localisation: a topography of auditory space. I. Normal hearing conditions. Perception 13(5):581–600Google Scholar
  45. Olivetti Belardinelli M, Santangelo V (2005) The head-centered meridian effect: auditory attention orienting in conditions of impaired visuo-spatial information. Disabil Rehabil 27(13):761–768Google Scholar
  46. Olivetti Belardinelli M, Santangelo V, Botta F, Federici S (2007) Are vertical meridian effects due to audio-visual interference? A new confirmation with deaf subjects. Disabil Rehabil 29(10):797–804Google Scholar
  47. Overholt EdwinM, Rubel EdwinW, Hyson RichardL (1992) A circuit for coding interaural time differences in the chick brainstem. J Neurosci 12(5):1698–1708Google Scholar
  48. Perrott DR (1984) Concurrent minimum audible angle: a re-examination of the concept of auditory spatial acuity. J Acoust Soc Am 75(4):1201–1206Google Scholar
  49. Perrott DR, Ambarsoom H, Tucker J (1987) Changes in head position as a measure of auditory localization performance: auditory psychomotor coordination under monaural and binaural listening conditions. J Acoust Soc Am 82(5):1637–1645Google Scholar
  50. Phillips DP (2008) A perceptual architecture for sound lateralization in man. Hear Res 238(1):124–132Google Scholar
  51. Pick HL, Warren DH, Hay JC (1969) Sensory conflict in judgments of spatial direction. Percept Psychophys 6(4):203–205Google Scholar
  52. Pierce AH (1901) Studies in auditory and visual space perception. Longmans, Green, and CompanyGoogle Scholar
  53. Pollack I, Rose M (1967) Effect of head movement on the localization of sounds in the equatorial plane. Percept Psychophys 2(12):591–596Google Scholar
  54. Populin LC (2008) Human sound localization: measurements in untrained, head-unrestrained subjects using gaze as a pointer. Exp Brain Res 190(1):11–30Google Scholar
  55. Preibisch-Effenberger R (1966) Endolaryngeale Ultraschallanwendung als neue Behandlungsmethode juveniler Kehlkopfpapillome. Eur Arch Oto Rhino Laryngol 186(2):146–152Google Scholar
  56. Razak KA (2011) Systematic representation of sound locations in the primary auditory cortex. J Neurosci 31(39):13848–13859Google Scholar
  57. Recanzone GH (2009) Interactions of auditory and visual stimuli in space and time. Hear Res 258(1):89–99Google Scholar
  58. Recanzone GH, Makhamra SD, Guard DC (1998) Comparison of relative and absolute sound localization ability in humans. J Acoust Soc Am 103(2):1085–1097Google Scholar
  59. Salminen NH, Tiitinen H, Yrttiaho S, May PJ (2010) The neural code for interaural time difference in human auditory cortex. ‎J Acoust Soc Am 127(2):EL60–EL65Google Scholar
  60. Slutsky DA, Recanzone GH (2001) Temporal and spatial dependency of the ventriloquism effect. NeuroReport 12(1):7–10Google Scholar
  61. Stevens SS, Newman EB (1936) The localization of actual sources of sound. Am J Psychol 48(2):297–306Google Scholar
  62. van Bergeijk WA (1962) Variation on a theme of Bekesy: a model of binaural interaction. J Acoust Soc Am 34(9B):1431–1437Google Scholar
  63. Von Békésy G, Wever EG (1960) Experiments in hearing, vol 8. McGraw-Hill, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  64. Wenzel EM, Arruda M, Kistler DJ, Wightman FL (1993) Localization using nonindividualized head-related transfer functions. J Acoust Soc Am 94(1):111–123Google Scholar
  65. Wightman FL, Kistler DJ (1989) Headphone simulation of free-field listening. II: psychophysical validation. J Acoust Soc Am 85(2):868–878Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Marta Olivetti Belardinelli and Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Lawrence Technological UniversitySouthfieldUSA

Personalised recommendations