Advertisement

Memory for positional movements as a component of the visuospatial working memory

  • Stefano SdoiaEmail author
  • Francesco Di Nocera
  • Fabio Ferlazzo
Short Communication
  • 34 Downloads

Abstract

Though the Corsi block-tapping task (CBT) is widely used for assessing visuospatial memory, information about what exactly it measures is still debated. We investigated such issue by observing how motor, visual, and spatial secondary tasks affect the performance on three versions of the CBT. Results showed a double dissociation pattern, wherein two motor secondary tasks had larger effects when the CBT was administered by the examiner tapping on the blocks. A spatial secondary task had larger effects when the CBT was administered by automatically illuminating the blocks. Finally, a visual secondary task had larger effects on a two-dimensional, computerized version of the CBT. These findings suggest that memory for movements plays a relevant role in the CBT, and are especially relevant due to their implications for assessment of brain-damaged patients, besides providing further evidence of a fractionation of visuospatial memory into multiple subcomponents.

Keywords

Corsi block-tapping task Visuospatial memory Memory for movements 

Notes

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in the present study were in accordance with the ethical standards of the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

All participants were informed verbally about the procedure and potential risks of their participation and gave their consent to participate to the study.

References

  1. Baddeley AD (2001) Is working memory still working? Am Psychol 56:849–864CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Baddeley AD, Hitch GJ (1974) Working memory. In: Bower GH (ed) The psychology of learning and motivation, vol 8. Academic Press, LondonGoogle Scholar
  3. Baddeley AD, Lieberman K (1980) Spatial working memory. In: Nickerson RS (ed) Attention and performance VIII. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, HillsdaleGoogle Scholar
  4. Baddeley AD, Logie R (1999) Working memory: the multiple component model. In: Miyake A, Shah P (eds) Models of working memory. Cambridge University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  5. Baddeley AD, Grant S, Wight E, Thomson N (1975) Imagery and visual working memory. In: Rabbitt PMA, Dornic S (eds) Attention and performance. Academic Press, LondonGoogle Scholar
  6. Berch DB, Krikorian R, Huha EM (1998) The Corsi block-tapping task: methodological and theoretical considerations. Brain Cogn 38:317–338CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bianchini F, Di Vita A, Palermo L, Piccardi L, Blundo C, Guariglia C (2014) A selective egocentric topographical working memory deficit in the early stages of Alzheimer's disease: a preliminary study. Am J Alzheimer's Dis Other Dement 29:749–754CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Brunetti R, Del Gatto C, Delogu F (2014) Corsi: implementation and testing of the Corsi block-tapping task for digital tablets. Front Psychol 5:939CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Brunetti R, Del Gatto C, Cavallina C, Farina B, Delogu F (2016) Did I see your hand moving? The effect of movement-related information on the Corsi block tapping task. Psychol Res.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-016-0834-6 Google Scholar
  10. Buneo CA, Andersen RA (2006) The posterior parietal cortex: sensorimotor interface for the planning and online control of visually guided movements. Neuropsychologia 44:2594–2606CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Carvalho CF, Menezes-Filo JA, de Matos VP, Bessa JR, Coelho-Santos J, Viana GFS et al (2014) Elevated airbourne manganese and low executive function in school-aged children in Brazil. NeuroToxicology 45:301–308CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Cavallini E, Fastame MC, Palladino P, Rossi S, Vecchi T (2004) Visuo-spatial span and cognitive functions: a theoretical analysis of the “Corsi” task. Imagin Cogn Personal 23:217–224CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Chiaravalloti ND, Glosser G (2004) Memory for faces dissociates from memory for location following anterior temporal lobectomy. Brain Cogn 54:35–42CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Claessen MH, Van Der Ham IJ, Van Zandvoort MJ (2015) Computerization of the standard Corsi block-tapping task affects its underlying cognitive concepts: a pilot study. Appl Neuropsychol Adult 22:180–188CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Corsi PM (1972) Human memory and the medial temporal region of the brain. Diss Abstr Int 34:819BGoogle Scholar
  16. Curtis CE, D’Esposito M (2004) The effects of prefrontal lesions on working memory performance and theory. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 4:528–539CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Deiber MP, Ibanez V, Sadato N, Hallet M (1996) Cerebral structures participating in motor preparation in humans: a positron emission tomography study. J Neurophysiol 75:233–247CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Della Sala S, Gray C, Baddeley A, Wilson L (1997) The Visual Patterns Test: A new test of short-term visual recall. Thames Valley Test Company, Feltham, SuffolkGoogle Scholar
  19. Dunn JC, Kirsner K (1988) Discovering functionally independent mental processes: the principle of reversed association. Psychol Rev 95:91–101CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Fecteau S, Tormos JM, Gangitano M, Théoret H, Pascual- Leone A (2010) Modulation of cortical motor outputs by the symbolic meaning of visual stimuli. Eur J Neurosci 32:172–177CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Finke K, Bublak P, Neugebauer U, Zihl J (2005) Combined processing of what and where information within the visuospatial scratchpad. Eur J Cogn Psychol 17:1–22CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Fischer MH (2001) Probing spatial working memory with the Corsi blocks task. Brain Cogn 45:143–154CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Goodale MA, Milner AD (1992) Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends Neurosci 15:20–25CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Hoshi E (2006) Functional specialization within the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: a review of anatomical and physiological studies of non-human primates. Neurosci Res 54:73–84CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Hoshi E, Tanji J (2004) Area-selective neuronal activity in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex for information retrieval and action planning. J Neurophysiol 91:2707–2722CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Joyce EM, Robbins TW (1991) Frontal lobe function in Korsakoff and non-Korsakoff alcoholics: planning and spatial working memory. Neuropsychologia 29:709–723CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Kemps E (2001) Complexity effects in visuo-spatial working memory: Implications for the role of long-term memory. Memory 9:13–27CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Kessels RP, van Zandvoort MJ, Postma A, Kappelle LJ, de Haan EH (2000) The Corsi block-tapping task: standardization and normative data. Appl Neuropsychol 7:252–258CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Klauer KC, Zhao Z (2004) Double dissociations in visual and spatial short term memory. J Exp Psychol Gen 133:355–381CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Lange KW, Robbins TW, Marsden CD, James M, Owen AM, Paul GM (1992) L-Dopa withdrawal in Parkinson’s disease selectively impairs cognitive performance in tests sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction. Psychopharmacology 107:394–404CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Logie RH (2003) Spatial and visual working memory: a mental workspace. In: Irwin D, Ross B (eds) Cognitive vision: the psychology of learning and motivation, vol 42. Elsevier, San Diego, pp 37–78CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Logie RH, Marchetti C (1991) Visuo-spatial working memory: visual, spatial or central executive? In: Cornoldi C, McDaniels MA (eds) Mental images in human cognition. Springer, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  33. Mammarella N, Cornoldi C, Donadello E (2003) Visual but not spatial working memory deficit in children with spina bifida. Brain Cogn 53:311–314CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Martin L, Tapper A, Gonzalez DA, Leclerc M, Niechwiej-Szwedo E (2017) The effects of task-relevant saccadic eye movements performed during the encoding of a serial sequence on visuospatial memory performance. Exp Brain Res 235(5):1519–1529CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Milner B (1971) Interhemispheric differences in the localization of psychological processes in man. Br Med Bull 27:272–277CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Owen AM, Stern CE, Look RB, Tracey I, Rosen BR, Petrides M (1998) Functional organization of spatial and nonspatial working memory processing within the human lateral frontal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U. S. A. 95:7721–7726CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Paccalin C, Jeannerod M (2000) Changes in breathing during observation of effortful actions. Brain Res 86:194–200CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Pearson DG, Sahraie A (2003) Oculomotor control and the maintenance of spatially and temporally distributed events in visuo-spatial working memory. Quart J Exp Psychol Hum Exp Psychol 56A:1089–1111CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Piccardi L, Iaria G, Ricci M, Bianchini F, Zompanti L, Guariglia C (2008) Walking in the Corsi test: Which type of memory do you need? Neurosci Lett 432:127–131CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Piccardi L, Iaria G, Bianchini F, Zompanti L, Guariglia C (2011) Dissociated deficits of visuo-spatial memory in near space and navigational space: evidence from brain-damaged patients and healthy older participants. Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 18:362–384CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Popp P, Wulff M, Finke K, Ruhl M, Brandt T, Dieterich M (2017) Cognitive deficits in patients with a chronic vestibular failure. J Neurol 264(3):554–563CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Quinn JG (2008) Movement and visual coding: the structure of visuo-spatial working memory. Cogn Process 9:35–43CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Quinn JG, McConnell J (1996) Indications of the functional distinction between the components of visual working memory. Psychol Beitr 38:355–367Google Scholar
  44. Rizzolatti G, Fadiga L, Gallese V, Fogassi L (1996) Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 3:131–141CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Rizzolatti G, Fogassi L, Gallese V (2001) Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nat Rev Neurosci 2:661–670CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Shah DS, Prados J, Gamble J, De Lillo C, Gibson CL (2013) Sex differences in spatial memory using serial and search tasks. Behav Brain Res 257:90–99CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Smyth MM, Pelky P (1992) Short-term retention of spatial information. Br J Psychol 83:359–374CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Smyth MM, Pendleton LR (1989) Working memory for movements. Quart J Exp Psychol 41A:235–250CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Smyth MM, Pendleton LR (1990) Space and movement in working memory. Quart J Exp Psychol 42A:291–304CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Smyth MM, Scholey KA (1994) Interference in immediate spatial memory. Mem Cogn 22:1–13CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Smyth MM, Pearson NA, Pendleton LR (1988) Movement and working memory: patterns and positions in space. Quart J Exp Psychol 40A:497–514CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Stephan KM, Fink GR, Passingham D, Silbersweig D, Ceballos-Baumann AO, Frith CD, Frackowiack RSJ (1995) Functional anatomy of the mental representation of upper extremity movements in healthy subjects. J Neurophysiol 73:373–386CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Stoffers D, Berendse HW, Deijen JB, Wolters ECH (2003) Deficits on Corsi’s Block tapping task in early stage Parkinson’s disease. Park Relat Disord 10:107–111CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Tresch MC, Sinnamon HM, Seamon JG (1993) Double dissociation of spatial and object visual memory: evidence from selective interference in intact human subjects. Neuropsychologia 31:211–219CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Ungerleider LG, Mishkin M (1982) Two cortical visual systems. In: Ingle DJ, Goodale MA, Mansfield RJ (eds) Analysis of visual behavior. The MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  56. van Asselen M, Kessels RPC, Sebastiaan FW, Neggers SFW, Kappelle LJ, Frijns CJM, Postma A (2006) Brain areas involved in spatial working memory. Neuropsychologia 44:1185–1194CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Vandierendonck A, Kemps E, Fastame MC, Szmalec A (2004) Working memory components of the Corsi blocks task. Br J Psychol 95:57–79CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Vecchi T, Richardson JT (2001) Measures of visuospatial short-term memory: the Knox cube imitation test and the Corsi blocks test compared. Brain Cogn 46:291–295CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Walter H, Bretschneider V, Groen G, Zurowski B, Wunderlich AP, Tomczak R, Spitzer M (2003) Evidence for quantitative domain dominance for verbal and spatial working memory in frontal and parietal cortex. Cortex 39:897–911CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Wilson FAW, Scalaidhe SPO, Goldman-Rakic PS (1993) Dissociation of object and spatial processing domains in primate prefrontal cortex. Science 260:1955–1958CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Woodin ME, Heil J (1996) Skilled motor performance and working memory in rowers: body patterns and spatial positions. Quart J Exp Psychol 49A:357–378CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Zimmer HD, Speiser HR, Seidler B (2003) Spatio-temporal working-memory and short-term object-location tasks use different memory mechanisms. Acta Psychol 114:41–65CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Marta Olivetti Belardinelli and Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of Rome “La Sapienza”RomeItaly

Personalised recommendations