Advertisement

The effect of perceptual fluency on overcoming the interference of the More A-More B intuitive rule among primary school students in a perimeter comparison task: the perspective of cognitive load

  • Ying Liu
  • Ru-De LiuEmail author
  • Jon R. Star
  • Jia Wang
  • Huimin Tong
Article
  • 49 Downloads

Abstract

Recent research has shown that reducing the perceptual fluency of shape processing can be an effective means for overcoming the interference of the More A-More B intuitive rule among grade 3 students in a perimeter comparison task. From the perspective of cognitive load, the current study focused on the mechanism of perceptual fluency on overcoming the interference of the More A-More B intuitive rule among grade 3 students in a perimeter comparison task. The existing studies have suggested that perceptual disfluency will inevitably increase ECL in the task-solving process and thus potentially detrimental toward learning. However, we could argue that overcoming the interference of the More A-More B intuitive rule in the disfluent condition could be interpreted as a simultaneous increase in cognitive GCL. Because of the theoretically complementary relationship between ECL and GCL, two experiments were designed to respectively examine ECL and GCL under different perceptual fluency conditions in the perimeter comparison task. Experiment 1 (N = 33) used a dual-task paradigm to examine participants’ ECL, manipulating the clarity of shapes. The results indicated that participants experienced significantly less interference from the More A-More B intuitive rule under the low-perceptual-fluency condition than under the high-perceptual-fluency condition, while ECL was significantly higher under the former condition than under the latter one. Experiment 2 (N = 72) explored GCL in the perimeter comparison task through a self-designed transfer test, using an identical manipulation method of perceptual fluency as in experiment 1. Compared to the high-perceptual-fluency group, participants in the low-perceptual-fluency group performed significantly better in the perimeter comparison task and transfer test. It was concluded that low perceptual fluency resulted in participants’ ECL while at the same time producing GCL during completion of the perimeter comparison task.

Keywords

More A-More B intuitive rule Perceptual fluency Cognitive load Perimeter comparison task 

Notes

References

  1. Alter, A. L., Oppenheimer, D. M., Epley, N., & Eyre, R. N. (2007). Overcoming intuition: metacognitive difficulty activates analytic reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 136(4), 569–576.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Author (under review). Overcoming the interference of the More A-More B intuitive rule among primary school students in a perimeter comparison task: the influence of perceptual fluency. Journal of Educational Psychology. Under review. Google Scholar
  3. Azhari, N. (1998). Using the intuitive rule “Same A-same B” in conservation tasks. Unpublished manuscript (in Hebrew).Google Scholar
  4. Babai, R., Levyadun, T., Stavy, R., & Tirosh, D. (2006a). Intuitive rules in science and mathematics: a reaction time study. International Journal of Mathematical Education in Science and Technology, 37(8), 913–924.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Babai, R., Levyadun, T., & Tirosh, R. S. D. (2006b). Intuitive rules in science and mathematics: a reaction time study. International Journal of Mathematical Education in Science & Technology, 37(8), 913–924.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Babai, R., Shalev, E., & Stavy, R. (2015). A warning intervention improves students’ ability to overcome intuitive interference. ZDM Mathematics Education, 47(5), 735–745.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Babai, R., Nattiv, L., & Stavy, R. (2016). Comparison of perimeters: improving students’ performance by increasing the salience of the relevant variable. ZDM, 48(3), 367–378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Baddeley, A. (1992). Working memory. Science, 255(5044), 556–559.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Bjork, R. A. (2013). Desirable difficulties perspective on learning. In H. Pashler (Ed.), Encyclopedia of the mind (Vol. 4, pp. 134–146). Thousand Oaks: Sage Publication.Google Scholar
  10. Brünken, R., Plass, J. L., & Leutner, D. (2003). Direct measurement of cognitive load in multimedia learning. Educational psychologist, 38(1), 53–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Brünken, R., Steinbacher, S., Plass, J., & Leutner, D. (2004). Assessment of cognitive load in multimedia learning using dual-task methodology. Experimental Psychology, 49(2), 109–119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. D’Amore, B., & Fandiño Pinilla, M. I. (2006). Relationships between area and perimeter: Beliefs of teachers and students. Mediterranean Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 5, 1–29.Google Scholar
  13. De Croock, M. B. M., Merriënboer, J. J. G., & Paas, F. (1998). High versus low contextual interference in simulation-based training of troubleshooting skills: effects on transfer performance and invested mental effort. Computers in Human Behaviour, 14(2), 249–267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Dean, A. L. (1976). The structure of imagery. Child Development, 47(4), 949–958.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Dean, A. L., & Deist, S. (1980). Children’s precocious anticipatory images of end states. Child Development, 51(4), 1040–1049.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. DeLeeuw, K. E., & Mayer, R. E. (2008). A comparison of three measures of cognitive load: evidence for separable measures of intrinsic, extraneous, and germane load. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(1), 223–234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Denes, G., & Pizzamiglio, L. (1999). Handbook of clinical and experimental neuropsychology (pp. 28–30). Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  18. Diemand-Yauman, C., Oppenheimer, D. M., & Vaughan, E. B. (2011). Fortune favors the bold (and the italicized): effects of disfluency on educational outcomes. Cognition, 118(1), 111–115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Eitel, A., Kühl, T., Scheiter, K., & Gerjets, P. (2014). Disfluency meets cognitive load in multimedia learning: does harder-to-read mean better-to-understand? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28(4), 488–501.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Evans, J. S. B. T. (2010). Intuition and reasoning: a dual-process perspective. Psychological Inquiry, 21(4), 313–326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Forgas, J. P. (1995). Mood and judgment: the affect infusion model (AIM). Psychological Bulletin, 117(1), 39–66.Google Scholar
  22. Gillard, E., Dooren, W. V., Schaeken, W., & Verschaffel, L. (2009). Dual processes in the psychology of mathematics education and cognitive psychology. Human Development, 52(2), 95–108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Jong, T. D. (2010). Cognitive load theory, educational research, and instructional design: Some food for thought. Instructional Science, 38(2), 105–134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (1999). Managing split-attention and redundancy in multimedia instruction. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 13(4), 351–371.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Kühl, T., & Eitel, A. (2016). Effects of disfluency on cognitive and metacognitive processes and outcomes. Metacognition and Learning, 11(1), 1–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Lehmann, J., Goussios, C., & Seufert, T. (2016). Working memory capacity and disfluency effect: an aptitude-treatment-interaction study. Metacognition and Learning, 11(1), 89–105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Magreehan, D. A., Serra, M. J., Schwartz, N. H., & Narciss, S. (2016). Further boundary conditions for the effects of perceptual disfluency on judgments of learning. Metacognition and Learning, 11(1), 1–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Mayer, R. E. (2001). Multimedia learning. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Oppenheimer, D. M. (2008). The secret life of fluency. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(6), 237–241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Osman, M., & Stavy, R. (2006). Development of intuitive rules: evaluating the application of the dual-system framework to understanding children’s intuitive reasoning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13(6), 935–953.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Paas, F., & Merriënboer, J. J. G. V. (1994). Instructional control of cognitive load in the training of complex cognitive tasks. Educational Psychology Review, 6(4), 351–371.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Paas, F., Van Gog, T., & Sweller, J. (2010). Cognitive load theory: new conceptualizations, specifications, and integrated research perspectives. Educational Psychology Review, 22(2), 115–121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Reber, R., & Schwarz, N. (1999). Effects of perceptual fluency on judgments of truth. Consciousness & Cognition, 8(3), 338–342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Reber, R., Schwarz, N., & Winkielman, P. (2004). Processing fluency and aesthetic pleasure: is beauty in the perceiver’s processing experience? Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8(4), 364–382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Schnotz, W., & Kürschner. (2007). A reconsideration of cognitive load theory. Educational Psychology Review, 19(4), 469–508.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Song, H., & Schwarz, N. (2008). Fluency and the detection of misleading questions: low processing fluency attenuates the Moses illusion. Social Cognition, 26(6), 791–799.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Stavy, R., & Babai, R. (2008). Complexity of shapes and quantitative reasoning in geometry. Mind Brain & Education, 2(2), 170–176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Stavy, R., & Babai, R. (2010). Overcoming intuitive interference in mathematics: insights from behavioral, brain imaging and intervention studies. ZDM the International Journal on Mathematics Education, 42(6), 621–633.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Stavy, R., & Tirosh, D. (2000). How students (mis-) understand science and mathematics: intuitive rules. Teachers College Press.Google Scholar
  40. Stavy, R., Tsamir, P., & Tirosh, D. (2002). Intuitive rules: the case of “More A-More B”. reconsidering conceptual change: issues in theory and practice. Netherlands: Springer.Google Scholar
  41. Stavy, R., Goel, V., Critchley, H., & Dolan, R. (2006). Intuitive interference in quantitative reasoning. Brain Research, 1073-1074(1), 383–388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Stull, A. T., & Mayer, R. E. (2007). Learning by doing versus learning by viewing: three experimental comparisons of learner-generated versus author-provided graphic organizers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99(4), 808–820.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Sweller, J. (2010). Element interactivity and intrinsic, extraneous, and germane cognitive load. Educational Psychology Review, 22(2), 123–138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Sweller, J., Merrienboer, J. J. G. V., & Paas, F. G. W. C. (1998). Cognitive architecture and instructional design. Educational Psychology Review, 10(3), 251–296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Sweller, J., Ayres, P., & Kalyuga, S. (2011). Cognitive load theory. New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Tirosh, D., & Tsamir, P. (2014). Intuition in mathematics education. Netherlands: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Winkielman, P., Schwarz, N., Fazendeiro, T. A., & Reber, R. (2003). The hedonic marking of processing fluency: implications for evaluative judgment. In J. Musch & K. C. Klauer (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation: affective processes in cognition and emotion (pp. 189–217). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  48. Ying, L., Liu, R. D., Star, J. Jia, W., Rui, Z., & Huimin, T. (2019). Overcoming the interference of the More A-More B intuitive rule among primary school students in a perimeter comparison task: the influence of perceptual fluency. Journal of Educational Psychology (in press)Google Scholar
  49. Yue, C. L., Castel, A. D., & Bjork, R. A. (2013). When disfluency is-and is not-a desirable difficulty: The influence of typeface clarity on metacognitive judgments and memory. Memory & Cognition, 41(2), 229–241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Instituto Superior de Psicologia Aplicada, Lisboa and Springer Nature B.V. 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  • Ying Liu
    • 1
  • Ru-De Liu
    • 2
    Email author
  • Jon R. Star
    • 3
  • Jia Wang
    • 4
  • Huimin Tong
    • 4
    • 5
  1. 1.School of EducationHebei Normal UniversityShijiazhuangChina
  2. 2.Beijing Key Laboratory of Applied Experimental Psychology, School of PsychologyBeijing Normal UniversityBeijingChina
  3. 3.Graduate School of EducationHarvard UniversityCambridgeUSA
  4. 4.Teachers’ CollegeBeijing Union UniversityBeijingChina
  5. 5.Moral Education DepartmentWanquan Primary schoolBeijingChina

Personalised recommendations