Advertisement

Animal Cognition

, Volume 22, Issue 6, pp 907–915 | Cite as

The use of a human’s location and social cues by Asian elephants in an object-choice task

  • Oraya Ketchaisri
  • Chomcheun Siripunkaw
  • Joshua M. PlotnikEmail author
Original Paper

Abstract

Asian elephants have previously demonstrated an ability to follow olfactory cues, but not human-provided social cues like pointing and gazing or orienting to find hidden food (Plotnik et al. in PLoS One 8:e61174, 2013; Anim Behav 88:91–98, 2014). In a study conducted with African elephants, however, elephants were able to follow a combination of these social cues to find food, even when the experimenter’s position was counter to the location of the food. The authors of the latter study argued that the differences in the two species’ performances might have been due to methodological differences in the study designs (Smet and Byrne in Curr Biol 23(20):2033–2037, 2013). To further investigate the reasons for these potential differences, we partially adapted Smet and Byrne (2013)’s design for a group of Asian elephants in Thailand. In a two-object-choice task in which only one of two buckets was baited with food, we found that, as a group, the elephants did not follow cues provided by an experimenter when she was positioned either equidistant between the buckets or closer to the incorrect bucket when providing the cues. The elephants did, however, follow cues when the experimenter was closer to the correct bucket. In addition, there was individual variability in the elephants’ performance within and across experimental conditions. This indicates that in general, for Asian elephants, the pointing and/or gazing cues alone may not be salient enough; local enhancement in the form of the experimenter’s position in relation to the food reward may represent a crucial, complementary cue. These results suggest that the variability within and between the species in their performance on these tasks could be due to a number of factors, including methodology, the elephants’ experiences with their handlers, ecological differences in how Asian and African elephants use non-visual sensory information to find food in the wild, or some combination of the three.

Keywords

Asian elephants Cognition Object-choice Visual cues Elephant behavior 

Notes

Acknowledgements

We thank the mahouts, staff and volunteers at Elephants World in Kanchanaburi, Thailand for their assistance with this study. We also thank the faculty and students of the Conservation Biology Program at Mahidol University’s Kanchanaburi Campus for their support. Finally, we appreciate the comments of anonymous reviewers that helped improve an earlier version of this manuscript.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

JP is the founder of Think Elephants International, a US public charity that focuses on elephant conservation. CS is co-founder of the Banana Orchard Project in Kanchanaburi, an ecotourism venture for captive elephants in Thailand. CS’s involvement in the latter project did not begin until after the current study was completed.

Ethical statement

All applicable international, national and/or institutional guidelines for the care and use of animals were followed.

Supplementary material

10071_2019_1283_MOESM1_ESM.mp4 (1.3 mb)
Movie 1. As an example of the general experimental setup, this movie shows a single trial of pointing + gazing from experiment 1. The elephant, Nimochi, chose incorrectly in this trial. (MP4 1320 kb)

References

  1. Bates LA, Sayialel KN, Njiraini NW, Moss CJ, Poole JH, Byrne RW (2007) Elephants classify human ethnic groups by odor and garment color. Curr Biol 17:1938–1942PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Bates LA, Sayialel KN, Njiraini NW, Poole JH, Moss CJ, Byrne RW (2008) African elephants have expectations about the locations of out-of-sight family members. Biol Let 4:34–36Google Scholar
  3. Call J (2004) Inferences about the location of food in the great apes (Pan paniscus, Pan troglodytes, Gorilla gorilla, and Pongo pygmaeus). J Comp Psychol 118(2):232–241PubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Call J, Agnetta B, Tomasello M (2000) Cues that chimpanzees do and do not use to find hidden objects. Anim Cogn 3:23–34Google Scholar
  5. de Silva S, Wittemyer G (2012) A comparison of social organization in Asian elephants and African savannah elephants. Int J Primatol 33(5):1125–1141Google Scholar
  6. Essler JL, Schwartz LP, Rossettie MS, Judge PG (2017) Capuchin monkeys’ use of human and conspecific cues to solve a hidden object choice task. Anim Cogn 20(5):985–998.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-017-1118-2 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Giret N, Miklósi A, Kreutzer M, Bovet D (2009) Use of experimenter-given cues by African gray parrots (Psittacus erithacus). Anim Cogn 12:1–10PubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Hare B (2001) Can competitive paradigms increase the validity of social cognitive experiments on primates? Anim Cogn 4:269–280PubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Hare B, Tomasello M (1999) Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) use human and conspecific social cues to locate hidden food. J Compar Psychol 113(2):173–177Google Scholar
  10. Hare B, Tomasello M (2004) Chimpanzees are more skillful in competitive than in cooperative cognitive tasks. Anim Behav 68:571–581Google Scholar
  11. Hare B, Brown M, Williamson C, Tomasello M (2002) The domestication of social cognition in dogs. Science 298(5598):1634–1636PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Hare B, Rosati A, Kaminski J, Bräuer J, Call J, Tomasello M (2010) The domestication hypothesis for dogs’ skills with human communication: a response to Udell et al. (2008) and Wynne et al. (2008). Anim Behav 79(2):e1–e6Google Scholar
  13. Herman LM, Abichandani SL, Elhajj AN, Herman EYK, Sanchez JL et al (1999) Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) comprehend the referential character of the human pointing gesture. J Comp Psychol 113:347–364PubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Itakura S, Anderson JR (1996) Learning to use experimenter-given cues during an object-choice task by a capuchin monkey. Curr Psychol Cogn 15:103–112Google Scholar
  15. Itakura S, Tanaka M (1998) Use of experimenter-given cues during object choice tasks by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), an orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus), and human infants (Homo sapiens). J Comp Psychol 112:119–126PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Kaminski J, Riedel J, Call J, Tomasello M (2005) Domestic goats, Capra hircus, follow gaze direction and use social cues in an object choice task. Anim Behav 69(1):11–18Google Scholar
  17. Krause MA, Udell MA, Leavens DA, Skopos L (2018) Animal pointing: changing trends and findings from 30 years of research. J Compar Psychol 132:326–345Google Scholar
  18. Lair RC (1997) Gone astray: the care and management of the Asian elephant in domesticity. Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations, RomeGoogle Scholar
  19. Maros K, Gácsi M, Miklósi Á (2008) Comprehension of human pointing gestures in horses (Equus caballus). Anim Cogn 11(3):457–466PubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Miklósi Á, Soproni K (2006) A comparative analysis of animals’ understanding of the human pointing gesture. Anim Cogn 9:81–93PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Moss CJ, Croze H, Lee PC (eds) (2011) The amboseli elephants: a long-term perspective on a long-lived mammal. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  22. Mulcahy NJ, Call J (2009) The performance of bonobos (Pan paniscus), chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) in two versions of an object choice task. J Compar Psychol 123:304–309Google Scholar
  23. Mulcahy NJ, Hedge V (2012) Are great apes tested with an abject object-choice task? Anim Behav 83:313–321Google Scholar
  24. Nawroth C, Ebersbach M, von Borell E (2014) Juvenile domestic pigs (Sus scrofa domestica) use human-given cues in an object choice task. Anim Cogn 17:701–713PubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Neiworth JJ, Burman MA, Basile BM, Lickteig MT (2002) Use of experimenter given cues in visual co-orienting and in an object-choice task by a new world monkey species, cotton top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). J Comp Psychol 116:3–11PubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. O’Connell-Rodwell CE (2007) Keeping an “ear” to the ground: seismic communication in elephants. Physiology 22:287–294PubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Pardo MA, Poole JH, Stoeger AS, Wrege PH, O’Connell-Rodwell CE, Padmalal UK, de Silva S (2019) Differences in combinatorial calls among the 3 elephant species cannot be explained by phylogeny. Behav Ecol.  https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arz018 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Plotnik JM, Lair R, Suphachoksahakun W, de Waal FBM (2011) Elephants know when they need a helping trunk in a cooperative task. Proc Natl Acad Sci 108(12):5116–5121PubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Plotnik JM, Pokorny JJ, Keratimanochaya T, Webb C et al (2013) Visual cues given by humans are not sufficient for Asian elephants (Elephas maximus) to find hidden food. PLoS One 8:e61174PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  30. Plotnik JM, Shaw RC, Brubaker DL, Tiller LN, Clayton NS (2014) Thinking with their trunks: elephants use smell but not sound to locate food and exclude nonrewarding alternatives. Anim Behav 88:91–98Google Scholar
  31. Plotnik JM, Brubaker DL, Dale R, Tiller LN, Mumby HS, Clayton NS (2019) Elephants have a nose for quantity. Proc Natl Acad Sci 10:10.  https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1818284116 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Proops L, Walton M, McComb K (2010) The use of human-given cues by domestic horses, Equus caballus, during an object choice task. Anim Behav 79(6):1205–1209Google Scholar
  33. Range F, Virányi Z (2015) Tracking the evolutionary origins of dog-human cooperation: the “Canine-Cooperation” Hypothesis”. Front Psychol 5:e1582Google Scholar
  34. Scheumann M, Call J (2004) The use of experimenter-given cues by South African fur seals (Arctocephalus pusillus). Anim Cogn 7:224–230PubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Schmitt MH, Shuttleworth A, Ward D, Shrader AM (2018) African elephants use plant odours to make foraging decisions across multiple spatial scales. Anim Behav 141:17–27Google Scholar
  36. Smet AF, Byrne RW (2013) African elephants can use human pointing cues to find hidden food. Curr Biol 23(20):2033–2037PubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Sokal RR, Rohlf FJ (1995) Biometry: the principles of statistics in biological research. WH Freeman and Co, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  38. Sukumar R (2003) The living elephants: evolutionary ecology, behaviour, and conservation. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  39. Tebbich S, Seed AM, Emery NJ, Clayton NS (2007) Non-tool-using rooks, Corvus frugilegus, solve the trap-tube problem. Anim Cogn 10(2):225–231PubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Tornick JK, Gibson BM, Kispert D, Wilkinson M (2011) Clark’s nutcrackers (Nucifraga columbiana) use gestures to identify the location of hidden food. Anim Cogn 14:117–125PubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Tschudin A, Call J, Dunbar RIM, Harris G, van der Elst C (2001) Comprehension of signs by dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). J Comp Psychol 115:100–105PubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Udell MAR, Dorey NR, Wynne CDL (2008) Wolves outperform dogs in following human social cues. Anim Behav 76:1767–1773Google Scholar
  43. Virányi Z, Gácsi M, Kubinyi E, Topál J, Belényi B, Ujfalussy D, Miklósi Á (2008) Comprehension of human pointing gestures in young human-reared wolves (Canis lupus) and dogs (Canis familiaris). Anim Cogn 11(3):373PubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. von Bayern AMP, Emery NJ (2009) Jackdaws respond to human attentional states and communicative cues in different contexts. Curr Biol 19:602–606Google Scholar
  45. Von Dürckheim KEM, Hoffman LC, Leslie A, Hensman MC, Hensman S, Schultz K, Lee S (2018) African elephants (Loxodonta africana) display remarkable olfactory acuity in human scent matching to sample performance. App Anim Behav Sci 200:123–129Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Conservation Biology ProgramMahidol University, Kanchanaburi CampusLum Sum, Sai Yok, Kanchanaburi 71150Thailand
  2. 2.Department of Psychology, Hunter CollegeCity University of New YorkNew YorkUSA

Personalised recommendations