Advertisement

Animal Cognition

, Volume 22, Issue 2, pp 243–250 | Cite as

Pet dogs exhibit social preference for people who synchronize with them: what does it tell us about the evolution of behavioral synchronization?

  • Charlotte DurantonEmail author
  • Thierry Bedossa
  • Florence Gaunet
Original Paper

Abstract

Humans show greater affiliation with people who are behaviorally synchronized with them but little is known about the impact of synchronization at an interspecific level. We, therefore, explored whether the synchronization of humans with dogs affects dogs’ human preferences. Pet dogs were exposed to two unfamiliar persons: one synchronized her walking behavior with them and one walked randomly. In a preference test, molossoids exhibited a clear social preference for the synchronized person, unlike shepherds. We conclude that pet dogs show a greater affiliation with humans who mimic their walking behavior, although genetic selection modulates this propensity. Behavioral synchronization, therefore, acts as a social glue in dogs too. It is the first time that such a human-like ability has been highlighted in domesticated canids at an interspecific level. Implications for the evolution of behavioral synchronization are discussed.

Keywords

Doghuman synchronization Preference test Behavioral synchrony Interspecific synchronization Dog mimicry 

Notes

Acknowledgements

The authors are grateful to Célicia Lecomte for her drawings and Elodie Losserand for the reliability coding. We are also grateful to Dr. Jessica Rock for her helpful edits and suggestions. We finally thank the owners who volunteered to take part in the study. This work was funded by the National Association for Research and Technology, the Help for Old Animals (AVA) association, the French National Center for Scientific Research, and Aix-Marseille University.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Asendorpf JB, Warkentin V, Baudonnière PM (1996) Self-awareness and other-awareness II: mirror-self recognition, social contingency awareness, and synchronic imitation. Dev Psychol 32(2):313–321Google Scholar
  2. Autier-Drian D, Deputte BL, Chalvet-Monfray K, Coulon M, Mounier L (2013) Visual discrimination of species in dogs (Canis familiaris). Anim Cogn 16(4):637–651Google Scholar
  3. Bornstein RF, D’Agostino PR (1992) Stimulus recognition and the mere exposure effect. J Pers Soc Psychol 63:545–552Google Scholar
  4. Charan J, Biswas T (2013) How to calculate sample size for different study designs in medical research? Ind J Psychol Med 35:121–126Google Scholar
  5. Chartrand TL, Bargh JA (1999) The chameleon effect: the perception-behavior link and social interaction. J Pers Soc Psychol 76(6):893–910Google Scholar
  6. Chartrand TL, Lakin JL (2013) The antecedents and consequences of human behavioral mimicry. Ann Rev Psychol 64:285308Google Scholar
  7. Cirelli LK, Wan SJ, Trainor LJ (2014) Fourteen-month-old infants use interpersonal synchrony as a cue to direct helpfulness. Phil Trans R Soc Lond B: Biol Sci 369:20130400Google Scholar
  8. Cirelli LK, Wan SJ, Trainor LJ (2016) Social effects of movement synchrony: increased infant helpfulness only transfers to affiliates of synchronously moving partners. Infancy 21:807–821Google Scholar
  9. Condon WS, Sander LW (1974) Synchrony demonstrated between movements of the neonate and adult speech. Child Dev 45:456–462Google Scholar
  10. Deldalle S, Gaunet F (2014) Effects of two training methods on stress-related behaviors of the dog (Canis familiaris) and on the dog-owner relationship. J Vet Behav Clin Appl Res 9(2):56–65Google Scholar
  11. Duffy DL, Hsu Y, Serpell JA (2008) Breed differences in canine aggression. Appl Anim Behav Sci 114:441–460Google Scholar
  12. Duranton C, Gaunet F (2015) Canis sensitivus: affiliation and dogs’ sensitivity to others’ behavior as the basis for synchronization with humans? J Vet Behav Clin Appl Res 10:513–524Google Scholar
  13. Duranton C, Gaunet F (2016) Behavioural synchronization from an ethological perspective: short overview of its adaptive values. Adapt Behav 24(3):181191Google Scholar
  14. Duranton C, Bedossa T, Gaunet F (2016) When facing an unfamiliar person, pet dogs present social referencing based on their owner’s direction of movement alone. Anim Behav 113:147156Google Scholar
  15. Duranton C, Bedossa T, Gaunet F (2017a) Do shelter dogs engage in social referencing with their caregiver in an approach paradigm? An exploratory study. Appl Anim Behav Sci 189:57–65Google Scholar
  16. Duranton C, Bedossa T, Gaunet F (2017b) Interspecific behavioural synchronization: dogs exhibit locomotor synchrony with humans. Sci Rep 7:12384Google Scholar
  17. Duranton C, Bedossa T, Gaunet F (2018) Pet dogs synchronize their walking pace with that of their owners in open outdoor areas. Anim Cogn 21(2):219–226Google Scholar
  18. Ebstrein RP, Israel S, Hong Chew S, Zhong S, Knafo A (2010) Genetics of human social behavior. Neuron 65(6):831–844Google Scholar
  19. Eken Asp H, Fikse WF, Nilsson K, Strandberg E (2015) Breed differences in everyday behavior of dogs. Appl Anim Behav Sci 169:69–77Google Scholar
  20. Frantz LAF, Mullin VE, Pionnier-Capitan M, Lebrasseur O, Ollivier M, Perri A, Larson G (2016) Genomic and archaeological evidence suggests a dual origin of domestic dogs. Science 352(6290):1228–1231Google Scholar
  21. Gaunet F, El Massioui F (2014) Marked referential communicative behaviours, but no differentiation of the “knowledge state” of humans in untrained pet dogs versus 1-year-old infants. Anim Cogn 17(5):1137–1147Google Scholar
  22. Guéguen N, Jacob C, Martin A (2009) Mimicry in social interaction: its effect on human judgment and behavior. Eur J Soc Sci 8(2):253259Google Scholar
  23. Hare B, Brown M, Williamson C, Tomasello M (2002) The domestication of social cognition in dogs. Science 298:1634–1636Google Scholar
  24. Harmon-Jones E, Allen JJB (2001) The role of affect in the mere exposure effect: evidence from psychophysiological and individual differences approaches. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 27(7):889–898Google Scholar
  25. Hill WF (1978) Effects of mere exposure on preferences in nonhuman mammals. Psychol Bull 85(6):1177–1198Google Scholar
  26. Jacob C, Guéguen N (2013) The effect of employees’ verbal mimicry. Int J Hosp Manag 35:109–111Google Scholar
  27. Kendon A (1970) Movement coordination in social interaction: Some examples described. Acta Psychol 32:100–125Google Scholar
  28. King AJ, Cowlishaw G (2009) All together now: behavioural synchrony in baboons. Anim Behav 78:1381–1387Google Scholar
  29. Kirschner S, Tomasello M (2010) Joint music making promotes prosocial behaviour in 4-year-old children. Evol Hum Behav 31:354–364Google Scholar
  30. Kis A, Bence M, Lakatos G, Pergel E, Turcsán B, Pluijmakers J, Vas J, Elek Z, Brúder I, Földi L, Sasvári-Székely M, Miklósi Á, Rónai Z, Kubinyi E (2014) Oxytocin receptor gene polymorphisms are associated with human directed social behavior in dogs (Canis familiaris). PLoS One 9:e83993Google Scholar
  31. Lakin JL, Jefferis VE, Cheng CM, Chartrand TL (2003) The chameleon effect as social glue: evidence for the evolutionary significance of nonconscious mimicry. J Nonverb Behav 27:145–162Google Scholar
  32. Ludbrook J, Dudley H (1998) Why permutation tests are superior to t and F rests in biomedical research. Am Stat 52(2):127–132Google Scholar
  33. Marshall-Pescini S, Dale R, Quervel-Chaumette M, Range F (2016) Critical issues in experimental studies of prosociality in non-human species. Anim Cogn 19(4):679–705Google Scholar
  34. Mehrkam LR, Wynne CDL (2014) Behavioral differences among breeds of domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris): current status of the science. Appl Anim Behav Sci 155:12–27Google Scholar
  35. Miklósi Á, Topál J, Csányi V (2007) Big thoughts in small brains? Dogs as a model for understanding human social cognition. Cogn Neurosci Neuropsychol 18(5):467–471Google Scholar
  36. Muller BCN, Maaskant AJ, van Baaren RB, Dijksterhuis A (2012) Prosocial consequences of imitation. Psychol Rep 110(3):891–898Google Scholar
  37. Nagasawa M, Mitsui S, En S, Ohtani N, Ohta M, Sakuma Y, Onaka T, Mogi K, Kikusui T (2015) Oxytocin-gaze positive loop and the coevolution of human-dog bonds. Science 348(6232):333–336Google Scholar
  38. Passalacqua C, Marshall-Pescini S, Barnard S, Lakatos G, Valsecchi P, Prato-Previde E (2011) Human-directed gazing behaviour in puppies and adult dogs, Canis lupus familiaris. Anim Behav 82:1043–1050Google Scholar
  39. Paukner A, Suomi SJ, Visalberghi E, Ferrari PF (2009) Capuchin monkeys display affiliation toward humans who imitate them. Science 325:880–883Google Scholar
  40. Quervel-Chaumette M, Dale R, Marshall-Pescini S, Range F (2016a) Familiarity affects other-regarding preferences in pet dogs. Sci Rep 5:18102Google Scholar
  41. Quervel-Chaumette M, Mainix G, Range F, Marshall-Pescini S (2016b) Dogs do not show pro-social preferences towards humans. Front Psychol 7: 1416.Google Scholar
  42. Richardson MJ, Marsh KL, Isenhower RW, Goodman JRL, Schmidt RC (2007) Rocking together: dynamics of intentional and unintentional interpersonal coordination. Hum Mov Sci 26:867–891Google Scholar
  43. Richardson DC, Dale R, Shockley K (2008) Synchrony and swing in conversation: coordination, temporal dynamics, and communication. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  44. Sanchez-Burks J, Bartel CA, Blount S (2000) Performance in intercultural interactions at work: cross-cultural differences in response to behavioral mirroring. J Appl Psychol 94(1):216–223Google Scholar
  45. Starling MJ, Branson N, Thomson PC, McGreevy PD (2013) Age, sex and reproductive status affect boldness in dogs. Vet J 197:868–872Google Scholar
  46. Svartberg K (2006) Breed-typical behaviour in dogs: historical remnants or recent constructs. Appl Anim Behav Sci 96:293–313Google Scholar
  47. Tunçgenç B, Cohen E (2016) Movement synchrony forges social bonds across group divides. Front Psychol 7:782Google Scholar
  48. Tunçgenç B, Cohen E, Fawcett C (2015) Rock with me: The role of movement synchrony in infants’ social and nonsocial choices. Child Dev 86(3):976–984Google Scholar
  49. Turcsán B, Kubinyi E, Miklósi Á (2011) Trainability and boldness traits differ between dog breed clusters based on conventional breed categories and genetic relatedness. Appl Anim Behav Sci 132:61–70Google Scholar
  50. van Baaren RB, Holland RW, Steenaert B, van Knippenberg A (2003) Mimicry for money: behavioral consequences of imitation. J Exp Soc Psychol 39(4):393–398Google Scholar
  51. van Baaren RB, Holland RW, Kawakami K, van Knippenberg A (2004) Mimicry and prosocial behavior. Psychol Sci 15(1):71–74Google Scholar
  52. van Ulzen NR, Lamoth CJC, Daffertshofer A, Semin GR, Beek PJ (2008) Characteristic of instructed and uninstructed interpersonal coordination while walking side-by-side. Neurosci Lett 432:88–93Google Scholar
  53. Vas J, Topál J, Gácsi M, Miklósi Á, Csányi V (2005) A friend or an enemy? Dogs’ reaction to an unfamiliar person showing behavioural cues of threat and friendliness at different times. Appl Anim Behav Sci 94:99–115Google Scholar
  54. Voss K, Galeandro L, Wiestner T, Haessif M, Montavon PM (2010) Relationships of body weight, body size, subject velocity, and vertical ground reaction forces in trotting dogs. Vet Surg 39(7):863–869Google Scholar
  55. Xavier J, Tilmont E, Bonnot O (2013) Children’s synchrony and rhythmicity in imitation of peers: toward a developmental model of empathy. J Phys Paris 107(4):291–297Google Scholar
  56. Zajonc RB (1968) Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. J Pers Soc Psychol Monogr 9(2:pt. 2):1–27Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Cognitive Psychology Laboratory (UMR 7290), 3C Research FederationAix-Marseille University, CNRSMarseilles Cedex 03France
  2. 2.AVA AssociationCuy-Saint-FiacreFrance
  3. 3.Alfort National Veterinary SchoolMaisons-AlfortFrance
  4. 4.Ethodog, Research in Canine EthologyMaisons-LaffitteFrance

Personalised recommendations