Advertisement

Animal Cognition

, Volume 22, Issue 3, pp 291–303 | Cite as

Guppies, Poecilia reticulata, perceive a reversed Delboeuf illusion

  • Tyrone Lucon-XiccatoEmail author
  • Maria Santacà
  • Maria Elena Miletto Petrazzini
  • Christian Agrillo
  • Marco Dadda
Original Paper

Abstract

Animals are often required to estimate object sizes during several fitness-related activities, such as choosing mates, foraging, and competing for resources. Some species are susceptible to size illusions, i.e. the misperception of the size of an object based on the surrounding context, but other species are not. This interspecific variation might be adaptive, reflecting species-specific selective pressures; according to this hypothesis, it is important to test species in which size discrimination has a notable ecological relevance. We tested susceptibility to a size illusion in the guppy, Poecilia reticulata, a fish species required to accurately estimate sizes during mate choice, foraging, and antipredator behaviours. We focussed on the Delboeuf illusion, in which an object is typically perceived to be larger when surrounded by a smaller object. In experiment 1, we trained guppies to select the larger of two circles to obtain a food reward and then tested them using stimuli arranged in a Delboeuf-like pattern. In experiment 2, we tested guppies in a spontaneous food choice task to determine whether the subjective size perception of food items is affected by the surrounding context. Jointly, our experiments indicated that guppies perceived the Delboeuf illusion, but in a reverse direction relative to humans: guppies estimated as larger the stimulus that human perceived as smaller. Our results indicated susceptibility to size illusions also in a species required to perform accurate size discrimination and support previous evidence of variability in illusion susceptibility across vertebrates.

Keywords

Comparative perception Fish cognition Size discrimination Visual illusion 

Notes

Acknowledgements

We thank the undergraduate students who helped in the project. We have no competing interests. Funding was provided by STARS 2017 Grant (Prot. 19215) and PRIN 2015 Grant (Prot. 2015FFATB7) from University of Padova.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

We have no conflicts of interest.

Ethical approval

We followed all applicable international, national, and/or institutional guidelines for the care and use of animals (Italy, D.L. 4 Marzo 2014, n. 26). All procedures performed in studies involving animals were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institution or practice at which the studies were conducted (Protocol n. 13/2018).

References

  1. Agrillo C, Miletto Petrazzini ME (2012) The importance of replication in comparative psychology: the lesson of elephant quantity judgments. Front Psychol 3:181Google Scholar
  2. Bánszegi O, Urrutia A, Szenczi P, Hudson R (2016) More or less: spontaneous quantity discrimination in the domestic cat. Anim Cogn 19:879–888CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Basolo AL (1990) Female preference for male sword length in the green swordtail, Xiphophorus helleri (Pisces: Poeciliidae). Anim Behav 40:332–338CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Birk MA, White JW (2014) Experimental determination of the spatial scale of a prey patch from the predator’s perspective. Oecologia 174:723–729CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bischoff RJ, Gould JL, Rubenstein DI (1985) Tail size and female choice in the guppy (Poecilia reticulata). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 17:253–255CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Byosiere SE, Feng LC, Woodhead JK, Rutter NJ, Chouinard PA, Howell TJ, Bennett PC (2017) Visual perception in domestic dogs: susceptibility to the Ebbinghaus–Titchener and Delboeuf illusions. Anim Cogn 20:435–448CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Cattelan S, Lucon-Xiccato T, Pilastro A, Griggio M (2017) Is the mirror test a valid measure of fish sociability? Anim Behav 127:109–116CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Cavoto KK, Cook RG (2001) Cognitive precedence for local information in hierarchical stimulus processing by pigeons. J Exp Psychol-Anim Behav Proces 27:3–16CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Croft DP, Darden SK, Ruxton GD (2009) Predation risk as a driving force for phenotypic assortment: a cross-population comparison. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 276:1899–1904CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Dadda M, Agrillo C, Bisazza A, Brown C (2015) Laterality enhances numerical skills in the guppy, Poecilia reticulata. Front Behav Neurosci 9:285CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Devigili A, Kelley JL, Pilastro A, Evans JP (2012) Expression of pre-and postcopulatory traits under different dietary conditions in guppies. Behav Ecol 24:740–749CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Dowds BM, Elwood RW (1983) Shell wars: assessment strategies and the timing of decisions in hermit crab shell fights. Behaviour 85:1–24CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Etheredge RI, Avenas C, Armstrong MJ, Cummings ME (2018) Sex-specific cognitive–behavioural profiles emerging from individual variation in numerosity discrimination in Gambusia affinis. Anim Cogn 21:37–53CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Evans JP, Kelley JL, Bisazza A, Finazzo E, Pilastro A (2004) Sire attractiveness influences offspring performance in guppies. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 271:2035–2042CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Feng LC, Chouinard PA, Howell TJ, Bennett PC (2017) Why do animals differ in their susceptibility to geometrical illusions? Psychon Bull Rev 24:262–276CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Fujita K, Matsuzawa T (1990) Delayed figure reconstruction by a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) and humans (Homo sapiens). J Comp Psychol 104:345–351CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Fuss T, Schluessel V (2017) The Ebbinghaus illusion in the gray bamboo shark (Chiloscyllium griseum) in comparison to the teleost damselfish (Chromis chromis). Zoology 123:16–29CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Gasparini C, Serena G, Pilastro A (2013) Do unattractive friends make you look better? Context-dependent male mating preferences in the guppy. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 280:20123072CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Gatto E, Lucon-Xiccato T, Savaşçı BB, Dadda M, Bisazza A (2017) Experimental setting affects the performance of guppies in a numerical discrimination task. Anim Cogn 20:187–198CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Hopkins WD (1997) Hemispheric specialization for local and global processing of hierarchical visual stimuli in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Neuropsychol 35:343–348CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Houde AE (1997) Sex, color, and mate choice in guppies. Princeton University Press, PrincetonGoogle Scholar
  22. Hunt E, Frost N, Lunneborg C (1973) Individual differences in cognition: a new approach to intelligence. Psychol Learn Motiv 7:87–122CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Karino K, Shinjo S (2004) Female mate preference based on male orange spot patterns in the feral guppy Poecilia reticulata in Japan. Ichthyol Res 51:316–320CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Kelley LA, Kelley JL (2014) Animal visual illusion and confusion: the importance of a perceptual perspective. Behav Ecol 25:450–463CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. King DL (1988) Assimilation is due to one perceived whole and contrast is due to two perceived wholes. New Ideas Psychol 6:277–288CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Lucon-Xiccato T, Bisazza A (2014) Discrimination reversal learning reveals greater female behavioural flexibility in guppies. Biol Lett 10:20140206CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Lucon-Xiccato T, Bisazza A (2017) Individual differences in cognition among teleost fishes. Behav Proc 141:184–195CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Lucon-Xiccato T, Dadda M (2017a) Individual guppies differ in quantity discrimination performance across antipredator and foraging contexts. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 71:13CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Lucon-Xiccato T, Dadda M (2017b) Personality and cognition: sociability negatively predicts shoal size discrimination performance in guppies. Front Psychol 8:1118CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Lucon-Xiccato T, Miletto Petrazzini ME, Agrillo C, Bisazza A (2015) Guppies discriminate between two quantities of food items but prioritize item size over total amount. Anim Behav 107:183–191CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Magurran AE (2005) Evolutionary ecology: the Trinidadian guppy. Oxford University Press, OxfordCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. McNamara JM, Houston AI, Collins EJ (2001) Optimality models in behavioral biology. SIAM Rev 43:413–466CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Miletto Petrazzini ME, Agrillo C (2016) Turning to the larger shoal: are there individual differences in small-and large-quantity discrimination of guppies? Ethol Ecol Evol 28:211–220Google Scholar
  34. Miletto Petrazzini ME, Bisazza A, Agrillo C (2017) Do domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) perceive the Delboeuf illusion? Anim Cogn 20:427–434CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Miletto Petrazzini ME, Parrish AE, Beran MJ, Agrillo C (2018) Exploring the solitaire illusion in guppies (Poecilia reticulata). J Comp Psychol 132:48–57CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Murayama T, Usui A, Takeda E, Kato K, Maejima K (2012) Relative size discrimination and perception of the Ebbinghaus illusion in a bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus). Aquat Mamm 38:333–342CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Nakamura N, Watanabe S, Fujita K (2008) Pigeons perceive the Ebbinghaus–Titchener circles as an assimilation illusion. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 34:375–387CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Nakamura N, Watanabe S, Fujita K (2014) A reversed Ebbinghaus–Titchener illusion in bantams (Gallus gallus domesticus). Anim Cogn 17:471–481CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Parrish AE, Beran MJ (2014) When less is more: like humans, chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) misperceive food amounts based on plate size. Anim Cogn 17:427–434CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Parrish AE, Brosnan SF, Beran MJ (2015) Do you see what I see? A comparative investigation of the Delboeuf illusion in humans (Homo sapiens), rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta), and capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn 41:395–405CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Parron C, Fagot J (2007) Comparison of grouping abilities in humans (Homo sapiens) and baboons (Papio papio) with the Ebbinghaus illusion. J Comp Psychol 121:405–411CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Pitteri E, Mongillo P, Carnier P, Marinelli L (2014) Hierarchical stimulus processing by dogs (Canis familiaris). Anim Cogn 17:869–877CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Prétôt L, Bshary R, Brosnan SF (2016a) Factors influencing the different performance of fish and primates on a dichotomous choice task. Anim Behav 119:189–199CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Prétôt L, Bshary R, Brosnan SF (2016b) Comparing species decisions in a dichotomous choice task: adjusting task parameters improves performance in monkeys. Anim Cogn 19:819–834CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Rahman MM, Kelley JL, Evans JP (2013) Condition-dependent expression of pre-and postcopulatory sexual traits in guppies. Ecol Evol 3:2197–2213CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Rodd FH, Hughes KA, Grether GF, Baril CT (2002) A possible non-sexual origin of mate preference: are male guppies mimicking fruit? Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 269:475–481CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Rosa Salva O, Rugani R, Cavazzana A, Regolin L, Vallortigara G (2013) Perception of the Ebbinghaus illusion in 4-day-old domestic chicks (Gallus gallus). Anim Cogn 16:895–906CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Santacà M, Regaiolli B, Miletto Petrazzini ME, Spiezio C, Agrillo C (2017) Preliminary study to investigate the Delboeuf illusion in ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta): methodological challenges. Anim Behav Cogn 4:365–377CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Sovrano VA, Albertazzi L, Rosa Salva O (2015) The Ebbinghaus illusion in a fish (Xenotoca eiseni). Anim Cogn 18:533–542CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Uller C, Lewis J (2009) Horses (Equus caballus) select the greater of two quantities in small numerical contrasts. Anim Cogn 12:733–738CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Ward C, Smuts BB (2007) Quantity-based judgments in the domestic dog (Canis lupus familiaris). Anim Cogn 10:71–80CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Watanabe S, Nakamura N, Fujita K (2011) Pigeons perceive a reversed Zöllner illusion. Cognition 119:137–141CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Watanabe S, Nakamura N, Fujita K (2013) Bantams (Gallus gallus domesticus) also perceive a reversed Zöllner illusion. Anim Cogn 16:109–115CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Life Sciences and BiotechnologyUniversity of FerraraFerraraItaly
  2. 2.Department of General PsychologyUniversity of PadovaPaduaItaly
  3. 3.School of Biological and Chemical ScienceQueen Mary University of LondonLondonUK

Personalised recommendations