Advertisement

Animal Cognition

, Volume 21, Issue 6, pp 787–794 | Cite as

Psychophysical investigation of vigilance decrement in jumping spiders: overstimulation or understimulation?

  • Bonnie Humphrey
  • William S. Helton
  • Carol Bedoya
  • Yinnon Dolev
  • Ximena J. Nelson
Original Paper

Abstract

The inability to maintain signal detection performance with time on task, or vigilance decrement, is widely studied in people because of its profound implications on attention-demanding tasks over sustained periods of time (e.g., air-traffic control). According to the resource depletion (overload) theory, a faster decrement is expected in tasks that are cognitively demanding or overstimulating, while the underload theory predicts steeper decrements in tasks that provide too little cognitive load, or understimulation. Using Trite planiceps, a jumping spider which is an active visual hunter, we investigated vigilance decrement to repetitive visual stimuli. Spiders were tethered in front of two stimulus presentation monitors and were given a polystyrene ball to hold. Movement of this ball indicates an attempt to turn towards a visual stimulus presented to a pair of laterally facing (anterior lateral) eyes for closer investigation with high acuity forward-facing (anterior median) eyes. Vigilance decrement is easily measured, as moving visual stimuli trigger clear optokinetic responses. We manipulated task difficulty by varying the contrast of the stimulus and the degree of ‘noise’ displayed on the screen over which the stimulus moved, thus affecting the signal:noise ratio. Additionally, we manipulated motivation by paired testing of hungry and sated spiders. All factors affected the vigilance decrement, but the key variable affecting decrement was stimulus contrast. Spiders exhibited a steeper decrement in the harder tasks, aligning with the resource depletion theory.

Keywords

Habituation Salience Salticidae Sustained attention Selective attention Vigilance 

Notes

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to Dr. Daniel Gerhard for help with the analysis. We also thank Aynsley McNab for technical assistance with maintenance of the spiders.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest and all applicable international, national, and institutional guidelines for the care and use of animals were followed.

Supplementary material

10071_2018_1210_MOESM1_ESM.docx (13.7 mb)
Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 13996 KB)

References

  1. Beauchamp G (2015) Overview of animal vigilance. In: Gomez KAS (ed) Animal vigilance: monitoring predators and competitors. Elsevier Science, New YorkCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Beauchamp G, Ruxton GD (2012) Changes in antipredator vigilance over time caused by a war of attrition between predator and prey. Behav Ecol 23:265–270CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bichot NP, Rossi AF, Desimone R (2005) Parallel and serial neural mechanisms for visual search in macaque area V4. Science 308:529–534CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Blake R, Logothetis NK (2002) Visual competition. Nat Rev Neurosci 3:13–21CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Broadbent DE (1965) Information processing in the nervous system. Science 150:457–462CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Claeskens G, Hjort NL (2008) Model selection and model averaging. Cambridge series in statistical and probabilistic mathematics. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Corbet PS (1999) Dragonflies: behaviour and ecology of Odonata. Cornell University Press, IthacaGoogle Scholar
  8. Dimond S, Lazarus J (1974) The problem of vigilance in animal life. Brain Behav Evol 9:60–79CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Duelli P (1978) Movement detection in the posterolateral eyes of jumping spiders (Evarcha arcuata, Salticidae). J Comp Physiol 124:15–26CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Dukas R (1998) Constraints on information processing and their effects on behaviour. In: Dukas R (ed) Cognitive ecology. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 89–127Google Scholar
  11. Dukas R (2002) Behavioural and ecological consequences of limited attention. Philos Trans R Soc Lond 357:1539–1547CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Dukas R (2004) Causes and consequences of limited attention. Brain Behav Evol 63:197–210CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Dukas R, Clark CW (1995) Sustained vigilance and animal performance. Anim Behav 49:1259–1267CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Esterman M, Reagan A, Liu G, Turner C, DeGutis J (2014) Reward reveals dissociable aspects of sustained attention. J Exp Psych: Gen 143:2287–2295CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Foelix RF (2011) Biology of spiders, 3rd edn. Oxford University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  16. Grier RA, Warm JS, Dember WN, Matthews G, Galinsky TL, Szalma JL, Parasuraman R (2003) The vigilance decrement reflects limitations in effortful attention, not mindlessness. Hum Factors 45:349–359CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Harland D, Jackson RR (2004) Portia perceptions: the Umwelt of an araneophagic jumping spider. In: Prete FR (ed) Complex worlds from simpler nervous systems. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 5–40Google Scholar
  18. Heisenberg M, Wolf R (1984) Vision in Drosophila: genetics of microbehaviour. Springer, BerlinCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Helton WS, Warm JS (2008) Signal salience and the mindlessness theory of vigilance. Acta Physiol (Oxf) 129:18–25Google Scholar
  20. Helton WS, Warm JS, Matthews G, Corcoran K, Dember WN (2002) Effects of signal salience and jet aircraft engine noise on performance and stress in an abbreviated vigilance task. Proc Hum Factors Ergon Soc 46:1456–1511Google Scholar
  21. Helton WS, Hollander TD, Warm JS, Matthews G, Dember WN, Wallaart M, Beauchamp G, Parasuraman R, Hancock PA (2005) Signal regularity and the mindlessness model of vigilance. Br J Psychol 96:249–261CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Herberstein ME (2011) Spider behaviour: flexibility and versatility. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Itti L, Koch C (2001) Computational modelling of visual attention. Nat Rev Neurosci 2:194–203CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Jackson RR, Harland D (2009) One small leap for the jumping spider but a giant step for vision science. J Exp Biol 212:2129–2132CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Kastner S, Ungerleider LG (2000) Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci 23:315–341CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Krause J, Godin JGJ (1996) Influence of prey foraging posture on flight behavioural and predation risk: predators take advantage of unwary prey. Behav Ecol 7:264–271CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Land MF (1985) Fields of view of the eyes of primitive jumping spiders. J Exp Biol 119:381–384Google Scholar
  28. Mackworth JF (1968) Vigilance, arousal, and habituation. Psychol Rev 75:308CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Melrose A, Nelson XJ, Dolev Y, Helton WS (2018) Vigilance all the way down: vigilance decrement in jumping spiders resembles that of humans. Q J Exp Psychol.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1747021818798743 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Milinski M (1990) Information overload and food selection. In: Hughes RN (ed) Behavioural mechanisms of food selection. Springer, Berlin, pp 721–737CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Nuechterlein KH, Parasuraman R, Jiang Q (1983) Visual sustained attention: image degradation produces rapid sensitivity decrement over time. Science 220:327–329CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Parasuraman R, Mouloua M (1987) Interaction of signal discriminability and task type in vigilance decrement. Percept Psychophys 41:17–22CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Pattyn N, Neyt X, Henderickx D, Soetens E (2008) Psychophysiological investigation of vigilance decrement: boredom or cognitive fatigue? Physiol Behav 93:369–378CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Paulk AC, Stacey JA, Pearson TWJ, Taylor GJ, Moore RJD, Srinivasan MV, van Swinderen B (2014) Selective attention in the honeybee optic lobes precedes behavioural choices. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 111:5006–5011CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Poggio T, Reichardt W (1976) Visual control of orientation behaviour in the fly. Part II. Towards underlying neural interactions. Q Rev Biophys 9:377–438CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Prete FR (1992) The effects of background pattern and contrast on prey discrimination by the praying mantis Sphodromantis lineola (Burr.). Brain Behav Evol 40:311–320CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Rankin CH, Abrams T, Barry RJ, Bhatnagar S, Clayton DF, Colombo J, Coppola G, Geyer MA, Glanzman DL, Marsland S, McSweeney FK, Wilson DA, Wu C-F, Thompson RF (2009) Habituation revisited: an updated and revised description of the behavioral characteristics of habituation. Neurobiol Learn Mem 92:135–138CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Rossel S (1980) Foveal fixation and tracking in the praying mantis. J Comp Physiol 139:307–331CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. R Core Team (2014) R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. http://www.R-project.org/ Google Scholar
  40. Sareen P, Wolf R, Heisenberg M (2011) Attracting the attention of a fly. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 108:7230–7235CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Sztarker J, Tomsic D (2011) Brain modularity in arthropods: individual neurons that support “what” but not “where” memories. J Neurosci 31:8175–8180CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Thompson RF, Spencer WA (1966) Habituation: a model phenomenon for the study of neuronal substrates of behaviour. Psychol Rev 73:16CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. van Swinderen B, Greenspan RJ (2003) Salience modulated 20–30 Hz brain activity in Drosophila. Nat Neurosci 6:579–586CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Warm JS (1984) Sustained attention in human performance. Wiley, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  45. Wilcox RS, Jackson RR, Gentile K (1996) Spiderweb smokescreens: spider trickster uses background noise to mask stalking movements. Anim Behav 51:313–326CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Zurek DB, Nelson. XJ (2012a) Hyperacute motion detection by the lateral eyes of jumping spiders. Vis Res 66:26–30CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Zurek DB, Nelson. XJ (2012b) Saccadic tracking of targets mediated by the anterior-lateral eyes of jumping spiders. J Comp Physiol 198:411–417CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Zurek DB, Taylor AJ, Evans CS, Nelson. XJ (2010) The role of the anterior lateral eyes in the vision-based behaviour of jumping spiders. J Exp Biol 213:2372–2378CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.School of Biological SciencesUniversity of CanterburyChristchurchNew Zealand
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyUniversity of CanterburyChristchurchNew Zealand
  3. 3.Department of PsychologyGeorge Mason UniversityFairfaxUSA

Personalised recommendations