Advertisement

Animal Cognition

, Volume 22, Issue 4, pp 473–486 | Cite as

How primates acquire their gestures: evaluating current theories and evidence

  • Katja LiebalEmail author
  • Christel Schneider
  • Manuela Errson-Lembeck
Original Paper
Part of the following topical collections:
  1. Evolving the study of gesture

Abstract

Mechanisms underlying gesture acquisition in primates are largely unstudied, yet heavily debated. While some studies suggest that gestural repertoires are largely innate, others emphasize that gestures emerge and are shaped in social interactions with other conspecifics. There is agreement, however, regarding the negligible role of imitation for the acquisition of gestures. The aim of this paper is to provide an overview of the current knowledge about gesture acquisition in nonhuman primates, to introduce the corresponding mechanisms suggested to be involved, and to discuss how findings from current studies support the different theories of gesture acquisition. We suggest that seemingly inconsistent findings across different research groups can be reconciled by pointing to differences between their research foci as well as methods of data collection. The additional comparison of the developmental pathways of gestural and facial communication highlights the complex interplay of genetic as well as social factors involved in shaping a species repertoire. We close by proposing that extending longitudinal studies, which capture the onset and usage of gestures in young primates, and which include the comparisons of several species and groups in different environments, will enable us to better understand developmental pathways of gestural communication in primates.

Keywords

Gestures Facial expression Genetic channeling Ontogenetic ritualization Imitation Social transmission Great apes 

Notes

Acknowledgements

We are very grateful to Michael Tomasello and Josep Call for giving permission to conduct observations and to analyze existing video footage at the Wolfgang-Köhler-Primate Research Center, Leipzig, used in some of the studies reported in this paper. We also want to thank the following zoos for their continued support during data collection: Apenheul and Burgers’ Zoo (Holland), Dierenpark Planckendael (Belgium), Zoo Leipzig, Zoo and Tierpark Berlin, Tierpark Hellabrunn, Allwetterzoo Muenster, Tiergarten Ulm, Zoo Wilhelma, Zoo Eberswalde, Zoo Osnabrück, Tierpark Jaderberg (Germany), ZSL London Zoo, Twycross Zoo, Drusillas Zoo Park, Noah’s Ark Zoo Farm, Belfast Zoo (United Kingdom), Mulhouse Zoo (France), Zurich Zoo (Switzerland), Dyrepark Kristiansand (Norway), Parken Zoo Eskilstuna (Sweden), and Randers Regnskov (Denmark).

Funding

This study was partly funded by Volkswagen Foundation “Towards a Grammar of Gesture: Evolution, Brain, and Linguistic Structures (ToGoG, #II/82175)”, and supported by the Freie Universität Berlin within the Excellence Initiative of the German Research Foundation.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All applicable international, national, and/or institutional guidelines for the care and use of animals were followed.

References

  1. Arbib MA (2005) From monkey-like action recognition to human language: an evolutionary framework for neurolinguistics. Behav Brain Sci 28(2):105–124.  https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X05000038 Google Scholar
  2. Arbib MA, Ganesh V, Gasser B (2014) Dyadic brain modelling, mirror systems and the ontogenetic ritualization of ape gesture. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 369(1644):20130414.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0414 Google Scholar
  3. Bard KA (1992) Intentional behavior and intentional communication in young free-ranging orangutans. Child Dev 62:1186–1197Google Scholar
  4. Bard KA (2003) Development of emotional expressions in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). In: Ekman P, Campos J, Davidson R, de Waal F (eds) Emotions inside out: 130 years after Darwin’s the expression of the emotions in man and animals. New York Academy of Sciences, New York, pp 88–90Google Scholar
  5. Bard KA (2005) Emotions in chimpanzee infants: the value of a comparative developmental approach to understand the evolutionary bases of emotion. In: Nadel J, Muir D (eds) Emotional development: recent research advances. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 31–60Google Scholar
  6. Bard KA, Dunbar S, Maguire-Herring V, Veira Y, Hayes KG, McDonald K (2014) Gestures and social-emotional communicative development in chimpanzee infants. Am J Primatol 76(1):14–29.  https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.22189 Google Scholar
  7. Bard KA, Maguire-Herring V, Tomonaga M, Matsuzawa T (2017) The gesture ‘touch’: does meaning-making develop in chimpanzees’ use of a very flexible gesture? Anim Cogn.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-017-1136-0 Google Scholar
  8. Bates LA, Byrne RW (2010) Imitation: what animal imitation tells us about animal cognition. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci 1(5):685–695Google Scholar
  9. Bates E, Benigni L, Bretherton I, Camaioni L, Volterra V (1979) The emergence of symbols: cognition and communication in infancy. Academic Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  10. Beisner BA, McCowan B (2014) Signaling context modulates social function of silent bared-teeth displays in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Am J Primatol 76(2):111–121Google Scholar
  11. Benga O (2005) Intentional communication and the anterior cingulate cortex. Interact Stud 6(2):201–221.  https://doi.org/10.1075/is.6.2.04ben Google Scholar
  12. Bourjade M, Meguerditchian A, Maille A, Gaunet F, Vauclair J (2014) Olive baboons, Papio anubis, adjust their visual and auditory intentional gestures to the visual attention of others. Anim Behav 87:121–128.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.10.019 Google Scholar
  13. Butterworth G (1998) What is special about pointing in babies? In: Simion F, Butterworth G (eds) The development of sensory, motor and cognitive capacities in early infancy. Psychology Press, East SussexGoogle Scholar
  14. Byrne RW, Tanner JE (2006) Gestural imitation by a gorilla: evidence and nature of the capacity. Int J Psychol Psychol Ther 6(2):215–231Google Scholar
  15. Byrne RW, Cartmill E, Genty E, Graham KE, Hobaiter C, Tanner J (2017) Great ape gestures: intentional communication with a rich set of innate signals. Anim Cogn 20(4):755–769Google Scholar
  16. Caeiro CC, Waller BM, Zimmermann E, Burrows AM, Davila-Ross M (2013) OrangFACS: a muscle-based facial movement coding system for orangutans (Pongo spp.). Int J Primatol 34(1):115–129.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-012-9652-x Google Scholar
  17. Call J (2001) Body imitation in an enculturated orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus). Cybern Syst 32(1–2):97–119Google Scholar
  18. Call J, Carpenter M (2002) Three sources of information in social learning. In: Dautenhahn K, Nehaniv CL (eds) Imitation in animals and artifacts. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  19. Call J, Tomasello M (1996) The effect of humans on the cognitive development of apes. In Russon AE, Bard KA, Parker ST (eds) Reaching into thought: the minds of the great apes. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 371–403Google Scholar
  20. Call J, Tomasello M (eds) (2007) The gestural communication of apes and monkeys. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  21. Camras LA, Oster H, Campos JJ, Bakemen R (2003) Emotional facial expressions in European–American, Japanese, and Chinese infants. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1000(1):135–151Google Scholar
  22. Cartmill EA, Byrne RW (2007) Orangutans modify their gestural signaling according to their audience’s comprehension. Curr Biol 17(15):1345–1348.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2007.06.069 Google Scholar
  23. Chevalier-Skolnikoff S (1982) A cognitive analysis of facial behavior in Old World monkeys, apes, and human beings. In: Snowdon CT, Brown CH, Peterson MR (eds) Primate communication. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 303–368Google Scholar
  24. Crockford C, Wittig RM, Mundry R, Zuberbühler K (2012) Wild chimpanzees inform ignorant group members of danger. Curr Biol 22(2):142–146.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2011.11.053 Google Scholar
  25. Custance DM, Whiten A, Bard K (1995) Can young chimpanzees imitate arbitrary actions? Hayes and Hayes (1952) revisited. Behaviour 132:839–858Google Scholar
  26. Darwin C (1872) The expression of emotion in animals and man. Murray, London, EnglandGoogle Scholar
  27. De Marco A, Visalberghi E (2007) Facial displays in young tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella): appearance, meaning, context and target. Folia Primatol 78(2):118–137Google Scholar
  28. Dean L, Vale GL, Whiten A (2016) Imitation, social learning and cultural traditions. In: Callan H (ed) International encyclopedia of anthropology. Wiley-Blackwell, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  29. Ekman P, Friesen WV (1978) Facial action coding system. Consulting Psychology Press, Palo AltoGoogle Scholar
  30. Fröhlich M, Wittig RM, Pika S (2016a) Play-solicitation gestures in chimpanzees in the wild: flexible adjustment to social circumstances and individual matrices. R Soc Open Sci 3(8):160278.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.160278 Google Scholar
  31. Fröhlich M, Wittig RM, Pika S (2016b) Should I stay or should I go? Initiation of joint travel in mother–infant dyads of two chimpanzee communities in the wild. Anim Cogn 19(3):483–500.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-015-0948-z Google Scholar
  32. Fröhlich M, Müller G, Zeiträg C, Wittig RM, Pika S (2017) Gestural development of chimpanzees in the wild: the impact of interactional experience. Anim Behav 134:271–282Google Scholar
  33. Galef BG (2013) Imitation and local enhancement: detrimental effects of consensus definitions on analyses of social learning in animals. Behav Proc 100:123–130Google Scholar
  34. Gasser B, Arbib MA (2018) A dyadic brain model of ape gestural learning, production and representation. Anim Cogn (this issue) Google Scholar
  35. Gasser B, Cartmill EA, Arbib MA (2014) Ontogenetic ritualization of primate gesture as a case study in dyadic brain modeling. Neuroinformatics 12(1):93–109Google Scholar
  36. Geissmann T (1984) Inheritance of song parameters in the gibbon song, analysed in two hybrid gibbons (Hylobates pileatus × H. lar). Folia Primatol 42(3):216–235.  https://doi.org/10.1159/000156165 Google Scholar
  37. Genty E, Breuer T, Hobaiter C, Byrne RW (2009) Gestural communication of the gorilla (Gorilla gorilla): repertoire, intentionality and possible origins. Anim Cogn 12(3):527–546.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-009-0213-4 Google Scholar
  38. Ghazanfar AA, Logothetis NK (2003) Facial expressions linked to monkey calls. Nature 423(6943):937–938.  https://doi.org/10.1038/423937a Google Scholar
  39. Ghazanfar AA, Takahashi DY, Mathur N, Fitch WT (2012) Cineradiography of monkey lip-smacking reveals putative precursors of speech dynamics. Curr Biol 22(13):1176–1182.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.04.055 Google Scholar
  40. Gillespie-Lynch K, Greenfield PM, Lyn H, Savage-Rumbaugh S (2014) Gestural and symbolic development among apes and humans: support for a multimodal theory of language evolution. Front Psychol 5:1228Google Scholar
  41. Graham KE, Furuichi T, Byrne RW (2017) The gestural repertoire of the wild bonobo (Pan paniscus): a mutually understood communication system. Anim Cogn 20(2):171–177Google Scholar
  42. Halina M, Rossano F, Tomasello M (2013) The ontogenetic ritualization of bonobo gestures. Anim Cogn 16(4):653–666.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-013-0601-7 Google Scholar
  43. Haun DB, Call J (2008) Imitation recognition in great apes. Curr Biol 18(7):R288–R290Google Scholar
  44. Hinde RA, Rowell TE (1962) Communication by postures and facial expressions in the rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta). Proc Zool Soc Lond 138(1):1–21Google Scholar
  45. Hobaiter C, Byrne RW (2011) The gestural repertoire of the wild chimpanzee. Anim Cogn 14(5):745–767.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-011-0409-2 Google Scholar
  46. Hobaiter C, Byrne RW (2017) What is a gesture? A meaning-based approach to defining gestural repertoires. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 82:3–12Google Scholar
  47. Hobaiter C, Byrne RW, Zuberbühler K (2017) Wild chimpanzees’ use of single and combined vocal and gestural signals. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 71(6):96Google Scholar
  48. Holodynski M, Friedlmeier W (2006) Development of emotions and emotion regulation. Springer, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  49. Iverson JM, Goldin-Meadow S (1998) The nature and functions of gesture in children’s communication. Jossey Bass, San FranciscoGoogle Scholar
  50. Kalan AK, Rainey HJ (2009) Hand-clapping as a communicative gesture by wild female swamp gorillas. Primates 50(3):273–275.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-009-0130-9 Google Scholar
  51. Kendon A (2004) Gesture: visible action. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  52. Kummer H (1997) In quest of the sacred baboon: a scientist’s journey. Princeton University Press, New JerseyGoogle Scholar
  53. Leavens DA (2004) Manual deixis in apes and humans. Interact Stud 5(3):387–407Google Scholar
  54. Leavens DA, Hopkins WD (1998) Intentional communication by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): a cross-sectional study of the use of referential gestures. Dev Psychol 34(5):813–822.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.34.5.813 Google Scholar
  55. Leavens DA, Russell JL, Hopkins WD (2005) Intentionality as measured in the persistence and elaboration of communication by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Child Dev 76(1):291–306.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00845.x Google Scholar
  56. Lembeck M (2015) Parenting behaviour in the first year of life: a cross-species comparison of humans, chimpanzees, bonobos and gibbons. Ph.D. Dissertation, Freie Universität Berlin, BerlinGoogle Scholar
  57. Liebal K (2017) What is a gesture? A lesson from comparative gesture research. Behav Brain Sci, 40Google Scholar
  58. Liebal K, Call J (2012) The origins of non-human primates’ manual gestures. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 367(1585):118–128.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2011.0044 Google Scholar
  59. Liebal K, Oña L (2018) Mind the gap—moving beyond the dichotomy between intentional gestures and emotional facial and vocal signals of nonhuman primates. Interact Stud (in press) Google Scholar
  60. Liebal K, Pika S, Tomasello M (2004) Social communication in siamangs (Symphalangus syndactylus): use of gestures and facial expressions. Primates 45(1):41–57.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-003-0063-7 Google Scholar
  61. Liebal K, Pika S, Tomasello M (2006) Gestural communication of orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). Gesture 6(1):1–38.  https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.6.1.02lie Google Scholar
  62. Liebal K, Waller BM, Burrows AM, Slocombe KE (2013) Primate communication: a multimodal approach. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  63. Liszkowski U, Carpenter M, Henning A, Striano T, Tomasello M (2004) Twelve-month-olds point to share attention and interest. Dev Sci 7(3):297–307.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00349.x Google Scholar
  64. Liszkowski U, Brown P, Callaghan T, Takada A, de Vos C (2012) A prelinguistic gestural universal of human communication. Cogn Sci 36(4):698–713.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1551-6709.2011.01228.x Google Scholar
  65. Lonsdorf EV, Markham AC, Heintz MR, Anderson KE, Ciuk DJ, Goodall J, Murray CM (2014) Sex differences in wild chimpanzee behavior emerge during infancy. PLoS One 9(6):e99099Google Scholar
  66. Maestripieri D (1999) Primate social organization, gestural repetoire size, and communication dynamics: a comparative study of macaques. In: King BJ (ed) The origins of language: what nonhuman primates can tell us. School of American Research Press, Santa Fe, pp 55–77Google Scholar
  67. Miles HL, Mitchell RW, Harper SE (1998) Simon says: the development of imitation in an enculturated orangutan. In Russon AE, Bard KA, Parker ST (eds) Reaching into thought: the minds of the great apes. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 278–299Google Scholar
  68. Mitchell GD, Raymond EJ, Ruppenthal GC, Harlow HF (1966) Long-term effects of total social isolation upon behavior of rhesus monkeys. Psychol Rep 18(2):567–580Google Scholar
  69. Mizuno Y, Takeshita H, Matsuzawa T (2006) Behavior of infant chimpanzees during the night in the first 4 months of life: smiling and suckling in relation to behavioral state. Infancy 9(2):221–240Google Scholar
  70. Myowa-Yamakoshi M, Matsuzawa T (1999) Factors influencing imitation of manipulatory actions in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J Comp Psychol 113(2):128–136Google Scholar
  71. Nielsen M, Collier-Baker E, Davis J, Suddendorf T (2005) Imitation recognition in a captive chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). Anim Cogn 8(1):31–36Google Scholar
  72. Owren MJ, Dieter JA, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (1992) Evidence of limited modification in the vocalizations of cross-fostered rhesus (Macaca mulatta) and Japanese (M. fuscata) macaques. In: Nishida T, McGrew WC, Marler P, Pickford M, de Waal F. B. M. (eds) Topics in primatology: human origins. University of Tokyo Press, TokyoGoogle Scholar
  73. Parr LA, Waller BM, Vick S-J (2007a) New developments in understanding emotional facial signals in chimpanzees. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 16(3):117–122.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00487.x Google Scholar
  74. Parr LA, Waller BM, Vick S-J, Bard K (2007b) Classifying chimpanzee facial expressions using muscles action. Emotion 7(1):171–181Google Scholar
  75. Parr LA, Waller BM, Burrows AM, Gothard KM, Vick S-J (2010) Brief communication: MaqFACS: a muscle-based facial movement coding system for the rhesus macaque. Am J Phys Anthropol 143(4):625–630Google Scholar
  76. Perlman M, Clark N, Tanner JE (2014) Iconicity and ape gesture. In: Cartmill E, Lyn H, Cornish H (eds) The evolution of language. Proceedings of the 10th international conference (EVOLANG 10). World Scientific, Singapore, pp. 228–235Google Scholar
  77. Piaget J (1952) The origins of intelligence in children (M. Cook Trans.). International University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  78. Pika S, Fröhlich M (2018) Gestural acquisition in great apes: the social negotiation hypothesis. Anim Cogn 1–15Google Scholar
  79. Pika S, Liebal K, Tomasello M (2003) Gestural communication in young gorillas (Gorilla gorilla): gestural repertoire, learning, and use. Am J Primatol 60(3):95–111.  https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.10097 Google Scholar
  80. Pika S, Liebal K, Tomasello M (2005) Gestural communication in subadult bonobos (Pan paniscus): repertoire and use. Am J Primatol 65(1):39–61.  https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20096 Google Scholar
  81. Plooij FX (1978) Some basic traits of language in wild chimpanzees? In: Lock A (ed) Action, gesture and symbol: the emergence of language. Academic Press, London, pp 111–131Google Scholar
  82. Plooij FX (1984) The behavioral development of free-living chimpanzee babies and infants. Ablex Publishing Corporation, New JerseyGoogle Scholar
  83. Pollick AS, de Waal FBM (2007) Ape gestures and language evolution. Proc Natl Acad Sci 104(19):8184–8189.  https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0702624104 Google Scholar
  84. Preuschoft S (2004) Power and communication. In: Thierry B, Singh M, Kaumanns W (eds) Macaque societies: a model for the study of social organization. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 56–60Google Scholar
  85. Redican WK (1975) Facial expressions in nonhuman primates. In: Rosenblum LA (ed) Primate behavior: developments in field and laboratory research, vol 4. Academic Press, New York, pp 103–194Google Scholar
  86. Redshaw M, Locke K (1976) The development of play and social behaviour in two lowland gorilla infants. J Jersey Wildl Preserv Trust. In: 13th annual report, pp 71–86Google Scholar
  87. Roberts AI, Vick S-J, Roberts SGB, Buchanan-Smith HM, Zuberbühler K (2012) A structure-based repertoire of manual gestures in wild chimpanzees: statistical analyses of a graded communication system. Evol Hum Behav 33(5):578–589.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2012.05.006 Google Scholar
  88. Rosati AG, Wobber V, Hughes K, Santos LR (2014) Comparative developmental psychology: how is human cognitive development unique? Evolut Psychol 12(2):448–473.  https://doi.org/10.1177/147470491401200211 Google Scholar
  89. Ross KM, Bard KA, Matsuzawa T (2014) Playful expressions of one-year-old chimpanzee infants in social and solitary play contexts. Front Psychol.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00741 Google Scholar
  90. Scheider L, Waller BM, Oña L, Burrows AM, Liebal K (2016) Social use of facial expressions in hylobatids. PLoS One 11(3):e0151733.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0151733 Google Scholar
  91. Schel AM, Townsend SW, Machanda Z, Zuberbühler K, Slocombe KE (2013) Chimpanzee alarm call production meets key criteria for intentionality. PLoS One 8(10):e76674.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0076674 Google Scholar
  92. Schneider C (2012) The development of gestural communication in non-human great apes. Ph.D. Dissertation, Freie Universität Berlin, Berlin. http://www.diss.fu-berlin.de/diss/receive/FUDISS_thesis_000000035744. Accessed 12 Feb 2017
  93. Schneider C, Call J, Liebal K (2012a) Onset and early use of gestural communication in nonhuman great apes. Am J Primatol 74(2):102–113Google Scholar
  94. Schneider C, Call J, Liebal K (2012b) What role do mothers play in the gestural acquisition of bonobos (Pan paniscus) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)? Int J Primatol 33(1):246–262.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-011-9570-3 Google Scholar
  95. Schneider C, Liebal K, Call J (2017) ‘Giving’ and ‘responding’ differences in gestural communication between nonhuman great ape mothers and infants. Dev Psychobiol 59(3):303–313Google Scholar
  96. Slocombe KE, Waller BM, Liebal K (2011) The language void: the need for multimodality in primate communication research. Anim Behav 81(5):919–924.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.02.002 Google Scholar
  97. Sroufe LA (1997) Emotional development: the organization of emotional life in the early years. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  98. Stenberg CR, Campos JJ (1990) The development of anger expressions in infancy. In: Stein N, Leventhal B, Trabasso T (eds) Psychological and biological approaches to emotion. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, pp 247–282Google Scholar
  99. Subiaul F (2016) What’s special about human imitation? A comparison with enculturated apes. Behav Sci 6(3):1–26.  https://doi.org/10.3390/bs6030013 Google Scholar
  100. Tennie C, Call J, Tomasello M (2012) Untrained chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) fail to imitate novel actions. PLoS One 7(8):e41548.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0041548 Google Scholar
  101. Tomasello M (2008) Origins of human communication. The MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  102. Tomasello M, George BL, Kruger AC, Farrar MJ, Evans A (1985) The development of gestural communication in young chimpanzees. J Hum Evol 14(2):175–186.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0047-2484(85)80005-1 Google Scholar
  103. Tomasello M, Gust D, Frost GT (1989) A longitudinal investigation of gestural communication in young chimpanzees. Primates 30(1):35–50.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02381209 Google Scholar
  104. Tomasello M, Savage-Rumbaugh S, Kruger AC (1993) Imitative learning of actions on objects by children, chimpanzees and enculturated chimpanzees. Child Dev 64(6):1688–1705Google Scholar
  105. Tomasello M, Call J, Nagell K, Olguin R, Carpenter M (1994) The learning and use of gestural signals by young chimpanzees: a trans-generational study. Primates 35(2):137–154Google Scholar
  106. Tomasello M, Call J, Warren J, Frost GT, Carpenter M, Nagell K (1997) The ontogeny of chimpanzee gestural signals: a comparison across groups and generations. Evol Commun 1(2):223–259.  https://doi.org/10.1075/eoc.1.2.04tom Google Scholar
  107. Tramacere A, Moore R (2016) Reconsidering the role of manual imitation in language evolution. Topoi 1–10Google Scholar
  108. van Lawick-Goodall J (1968a) The behavior of free-living chimpanzees in the Gombe Stream Reserve. Anim Behav Monogr 1(3):161–311Google Scholar
  109. van Lawick-Goodall J (1968b) A preliminary report on expressive movements and communication in the gombe stream chimpanzees. In: Jay PC (ed) Primates. Studies in adaptation and variability. Holt, Rinehort & Winston, New York, pp 313–374Google Scholar
  110. van Noordwijk MA, van Schaik CP (2009) Intersexual food transfer among orangutans: do females test males for coercive tendency? Behav Ecol Sociobiol 63(6):883–890.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-009-0728-3 Google Scholar
  111. Vick SJ, Waller BM, Parr LA, Smith Pasqualini MC, Bard KA (2007) A cross-species comparison of facial morphology and movement in humans and chimpanzees using the facial action coding system (FACS). J Nonverbal Behav 31(1):1–20Google Scholar
  112. Waller BM, Dunbar RIM (2005) Differential behavioural effects of silent bared teeth display and relaxed open mouth display in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Ethology 111(2):129–142Google Scholar
  113. Waller BM, Micheletta J (2013) Facial expression in nonhuman animals. Emot Rev 5(1):54–59Google Scholar
  114. Waller BM, Lembeck M, Kuchenbuch P, Burrows AM, Liebal K (2012) GibbonFACS: a muscle-based facial movement coding system for hylobatids. Int J Primatol 33(4):809–821Google Scholar
  115. Waller BM, Caeiro CC, Davila-Ross M (2015) Orangutans modify facial displays depending on recipient attention. PeerJ 3:e827.  https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.827 Google Scholar
  116. Whiten A, Ham R (1992) On the nature and evolution of imitation in the animal kingdom: Reappraisal of a century of research. In: Slater P. J. B., Beer R. J.S., C., Milinski M (eds) Advances in the study of behaviour, vol 21. Academic Press, New York, pp 239–283Google Scholar
  117. Wich SA, Utami-Atmoko SS, Mitra S, Rijksen T, Schürmann HD, van Hooff C, R. A. M. JA, van Schaik CP (2004) Life history of wild Sumatran orangutans (Pongo abelii). J Hum Evol 47(6):385–398.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhevol.2004.08.006 Google Scholar
  118. Wilke C, Kavanagh E, Donnellan E, Waller BM, Machanda ZP, Slocombe KE (2017) Production of and responses to unimodal and multimodal signals in wild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii. Anim Behav 123:305–316.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.10.024 Google Scholar
  119. Winter P, Handley P, Ploog D, Schott D (1973) Ontogeny of squirrel monkey calls under normal conditions and under acoustic isolation. Behaviour 47(3):230–239Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Education and Psychology, Comparative Developmental PsychologyFreie Universität BerlinBerlinGermany
  2. 2.School of PsychologyUniversity of East LondonLondonUK

Personalised recommendations