Advertisement

Animal Cognition

, Volume 15, Issue 1, pp 107–120 | Cite as

Waiting for more: the performance of domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) on exchange tasks

  • Rebecca J. LeonardiEmail author
  • Sarah-Jane Vick
  • Valérie Dufour
Original Paper

Abstract

Five domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) were tested in a cooperative exchange task with an experimenter, as previously tested in non-human primates. In the first task, the dogs exchanged to maximise payoffs when presented with food items of differing quality. All consistently exchanged lower-value for higher-value rewards, as determined by their individual food preference, and exchanges corresponded significantly with the spontaneous preferences of three dogs. Next, all subjects demonstrated an ability to perform two and three exchanges in succession, to gain both qualitative and quantitatively increased rewards (group mean = 72 and 92% successful triple exchanges, respectively). Finally, the ability to delay gratification over increasing intervals was tested; the dogs kept one food item to exchange later for a larger item. As previously reported in non-human primates, there was considerable individual variation in the tolerance of delays, between 10 s and 10 min for the largest rewards. For those who reached longer time lags (>40 s), the dogs gave up the chance to exchange earlier than expected by each subject’s general waiting capacity; the dogs anticipated delay duration and made decisions according to the relative reward values offered. Compared to primates, dogs tolerated relatively long delays for smaller value rewards, suggesting that the socio-ecological history of domestic dogs facilitates their performance on decision-making and delay of gratification tasks.

Keywords

Delay of gratification Reciprocity Cooperation Exchange Domestic dogs 

Notes

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Han de Vries (University of Utrecht) for devising the procedure used in the analysis of error times and Peter Cahusac for his help with additional statistical analyses. We are also grateful to the owners of the dogs involved in the study, Yvonne Bissell, and Lynne, Adrian and Bethan Hallgarth. We would also like to thank Jeff Stevens for helpful comments on an earlier version of this paper. Part of this research was funded by the Agence Nationale de la Recherche (ANR-08-Blanc-0042-01).

Supplementary material

10071_2011_437_MOESM1_ESM.doc (28 kb)
Supplementary material 1 (DOC 28.5 kb)

Supplementary material 2 (WMV 2201 kb)

Supplementary material 3 (WMV 4209 kb)

Supplementary material 4 (WMV 2802 kb)

Supplementary material 5 (WMV 9588 kb)

References

  1. Amici F, Aureli F, Call J (2008) Fission-fusion dynamics, behavioural flexibility and inhibitory control in primates. Curr Biol 18:1415–1419. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.08.020 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour (2006) Guidelines for the treatment of animals in behavioural research and teaching. Anim Behav 71:245–253. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.10.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Beran MJ (2002) Maintenance of self-imposed delay of gratification by four chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and an orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus). J Gen Psych 129:49–66. doi: 10.1080/00221300209602032 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Beran MJ, Evans TA (2006) Maintenance of delay of gratification by four chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): The effects of delayed reward visibility, experimenter presence, and extended delay intervals. Behav Proc 73:315–324CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Beran MJ, Savage-Rumbaugh ES, Pate JL, Rumbaugh DM (1999) Delay of gratification in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Dev Psychobiol 34:119–127PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bland MJ, Altman DG (1998) Statistics notes: survival probabilities (the Kaplan–Meier method). BMJ 317:1572–1580PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bond A, Kamil A, Balda R (2003) Social complexity and transitive inference in corvids. Anim Behav 65:479–487. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2003.2101 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bräuer J, Kaminski J, Riedel J, Call J, Tomasello M (2006) Making inferences about the location of hidden food: social dog, causal ape. J Comp Psychol 120:38–47. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.120.1.38 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Brosnan SF, Beran MJ (2009) Trading behavior between conspecifics in chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes. J Comp Psychol 123:181–194. doi: 10.1037/a0015092 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Byrne RW, Bates LA (2007) Sociality, evolution and cognition. Curr Biol 17:R714–R723. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.05.069 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Call J, Bräuer J, Kaminski J, Tomasello M (2003) Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) are sensitive to the attentional state of humans. J Comp Psychol 117:257–263. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.117.3.257 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Cools A, Van Hout A, Nelissen M (2008) Canine reconciliation and third-party-initiated postconflict affiliation: do peacemaking social mechanisms in dogs rival those of higher primates? Ethology 114:53–63. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2008.01485 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Cordoni G, Palagi E (2008) Reconciliation in wolves (Canis lupus): new evidence for a comparative perspective. Ethology 114:298–308. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2008.01474.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Csanyi V (2006) If dogs could talk: exploring the canine mind. Sutton, GloucesterGoogle Scholar
  15. deVries H, Netto WJ, Hanegraaf PLH (1993) Matman: a program for the analyses of sociometric matrices and behavioural transition matrices. Behav 125:157–175CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Drapier M, Chauvin C, Dufour V, Uhlrich P, Thierry B (2005) Food-exchange with humans in brown capuchin monkeys. Primates 46:241–248. doi: 10.1007/s10329-005-0132-1 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Drea C, Carter A (2009) Cooperative problem solving in a social carnivore. Anim Behav 78:967–977. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.06.030 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Dufour V, Sterck E (2008) Chimpanzees fail to plan in an exchange task but succeed in a tool-using procedure. Behav Proc 79:19–27. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2008.04.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Dufour V, Pelé M, Sterck E, Thierry B (2007) Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) anticipation of food return: coping with waiting time in an exchange task. J Comp Psychol 121:145–155. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.121.2.145 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Dunbar RIM (2009) Darwin and the ghost of Phineas Gage: neuro-evolution and the social brain. Cortex 45:119–1125. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.05.005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Dunbar RIM, Bever J (1998) Neocortex size predicts group size in carnivores and some insectivores. Ethology 104:695–708. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1998.tb00103.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Dunbar RIM, Shultz S (2007) Evolution in the social brain. Science 317:1344–1347. doi: 10.1126/science.1145463 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Emery N, Clayton N (2004) The mentality of crows: convergent evolution of intelligence in corvids and apes. Science 306:1903–1907. doi: 10.1126/science.1098410 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Evans TA, Beran MJ (2007) Chimpanzees use self-distraction to cope with impulsivity. Biol Lett 3:599–602. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2007.0399 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Gansloßer U (2009) Dynamics of conflict management in canines: preliminary results. J Vet Behav 4:101. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2008.09.002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Haccou P, Meelis E (1992) Statistical analysis of behavioural data. Oxford, Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
  27. Hare B, Brown M, Williamson C, Tomasello M (2002) The domestication of social cognition in dogs. Science 298:1634–1636. doi: 10.1126/science.1072702 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Hare B, Plyusnina I, Iganacio N, Wrangham R, Trut L (2005) Social cognitive evolution in captive foxes is a correlated by-product of experimental domestication. Curr Biol 15:226–230. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.01.040 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Hennessy MB, Williams MT, Miller DD, Douglas CW, Voith VL (1998) Influence of male and female petters on plasma cortisol and behaviour: can human interaction reduce the stress of dogs in a public animal shelter? App Anim Behav Sci 61:63–77. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(98)00179-8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Hennessy MB, Voith VL, Young TL, Hawke JL, Centrone J, McDowell AL, Davenport GM (2002a) Exploring human interaction and diet effects on the behavior of dogs in a public animal shelter. J Appl Anim Welfare Sci 5:253–273. doi: 10.1207/S15327604JAWS0504_01 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Hennessy MB, Voith VL, Hawke JL et al (2002b) Diet and human interaction moderate hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal responses of dogs in an animal shelter. J American Vet Med Assoc 221:65–71. doi: 10.2460/javma.2002.221.65 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Horowitz A (2009) Attention to attention in domestic dog (Canis familiaris) dyadic play. Anim Cogn 12:107–118. doi: 10.1007/s10071-008-0175-y PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Kacelnik A (2003) The evolution of patience. In: Lowienstein G, Read D, Baumeister R (eds) Time and decision: economic and psychological perspectives on intertemporal choice. Russell Sage Foundation, New York, pp 115–138Google Scholar
  34. Kubinyi E, Miklósi Á, Topál J, Csányi V (2003) Social mimetic behaviour and social anticipation in dogs: preliminary results. Anim Cogn 6:57–63. doi: 10.1007/s10071-003-0163-1 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Kubinyi E, Virányi Z, Miklósi Á (2007) Comparative social cognition: from wolf and dog to humans. Comp Cogn Behav Rev 2:26–46Google Scholar
  36. Kundey SMA, De LR, Taglang C, Allen R, Molina S, Royer E, German R (2010) Domesticated dogs (Canis familiaris) react to what others can and cannot hear. Appl Anim Behav Sci 126:45–50. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2010.06.002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Kupán K, Miklósi Á, Gergely G, Topál J (2011) Why do dogs (canis familiaris) select the empty container in an observational learning task? Anim Cogn 14:259–268. doi: 10.1007/s10071-010-0359-0 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Marshall-Pescini S, Valsecchi P, Petak I, Accorsi P, Previde E (2008) Does training make you smarter? The effects of training on dogs’ performance (Canis familiaris) in a problem solving task. Behav Proc 78:449–454. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2008.02.022 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Mazur JE (1987) An adjusting procedure for studying delayed reinforcement: the effects of delay and intervening events on reinforcement value. In: Commons ML, Mazur JE, Nevin JA, Rachlin H (eds) Quantitative analyses of behaviour 5. Erlbaum, Hillslade, pp 55–73Google Scholar
  40. Miklosi A (2007) Dog behaviour evolution and cognition. Oxford University Press, OxfordCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Mischel W (1974) Processes in delay of gratification. In: Berkowitz L (ed) Advances in experimental social psychology. Academic Press, New York, pp 249–292Google Scholar
  42. Mischel W, Shoda Y, Rodriguez ML (1989) Delay of gratification in children. Science 244:933–938. doi: 10.1126/science.2658056 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Möslinger H, Kotrschal K, Huber L, Range F, Virányi Z (2009) Cooperative string-pulling in wolves. J Vet Behav 4:99. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2008.09.030 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Mulcahy N, Call J (2006) Apes save tools for future use. Science 312:1038–1040. doi: 10.1126/science.1125456 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Naderi S, Miklósi Á, Dóka A, Csányi V (2001) Co-operative interactions between blind persons and their dogs. Appl Anim Behav Sci 74:59–80. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(01)00152-6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Palagi E, Cordoni G (2009) Postconflict third-party affiliation in Canis lupus: do wolves share similarities with the great apes? Anim Behav 78:979–986. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.07.017 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Pelé M, Micheletta J, Uhlrich P, Thierry B, Dufour V (2010a) Delay maintenance in Tonkean macaques and brown capuchin monkeys. Int J Primatol 32:149–166. doi: 10.1007/s10764-010-9446-y CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Pelé M, Dufour V, Micheletta J, Thierry B (2010b) Long-tailed macaques display unexpected waiting abilities in exchange tasks. Anim Cogn 13:263–271. doi: 10.1007/s10071-009-0264-6 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Prato-Previde E, Marshall-Pescini S, Valsecchi P (2008) Is your choice my choice? The owners’ effect on pet dogs’ (Canis lupus familiaris) performance in a food choice task. Anim Cogn 11:167–174. doi: 10.1007/s10071-007-0102-7 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Ramseyer A, Pele M, Dufour V, Chauvin C, Thierry B (2006) Accepting loss: the temporal limits of reciprocity in brown capuchin monkeys. Proc R Soc B 273:179–184. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2005.3300 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Range F, Heucke SL, Gruber C, Konz A, Huber L, Virányi Z (2009) The effect of ostensive cues on dogs’ performance in a manipulative social learning task. Appl Anim Behav Sci 120:170–178. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2009.05.012 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Rooney N, Bradshaw J (2006) Social cognition in the domestic dog: behaviour of spectators towards participants in interspecific games. Anim Behav 72:343–352. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.10.014 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Rosati AG, Stevens JR, Hare B, Hauser MD (2007) The evolutionary origins of human patience: temporal preferences in chimpanzees, bonobos, and human adults. Curr Biol 17:1663–1668. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.08.033 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Roth G, Dicke U (2005) Evolution of the brain and intelligence. Trends Cogn Sci 9:250–257. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.03.005 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Shifferman EM (2009) Its own reward: lessons to be drawn from the reversed-reward contingency paradigm. Anim Cogn 12:547–558. doi: 10.1007/s10071-009-0215-2 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Silverberg A, Widholm JJ, Bresler D, Fujita K, Anderson JR (1998) Natural choice in nonhuman primates. J Exp Psych Anim B 24:215–228. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.24.2.215 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Stevens JR, Hallinan EV, Hauser MD (2005) The ecology and evolution of patience in two New World monkeys. Biol Lett 1:223–226. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2004.0285 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Topál J, Miklósi Á, Csányi V (1997) Dog–human relationship affects problem-solving behavior in the dog. Anthrozoös 10:214–224. doi: 10.2752/089279397787000987 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Tuber DS, Hennessy MB, Sanders S, Miller JA (1996) Behavioral and glucocorticoid responses of adult domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) to companionship and social separation. J Comp Psychol 110:103–108. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.110.1.103 Google Scholar
  60. Vick SJ, Bovet D, Anderson JR (2010) How do African grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus) perform on a delay of gratification task? Anim Cogn 13:351–358. doi: 10.1007/s10071-009-0284-2 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Westergaard GC, Liv C, Rocca AM, Cleveland A, Suomi SJ (2004) Tufted capuchins (Cebus apella) attribute value to foods and tools during voluntary exchanges with humans. Anim Cogn 7:19–24. doi: 10.1007/s10071-003-0181-z PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  • Rebecca J. Leonardi
    • 1
    Email author
  • Sarah-Jane Vick
    • 1
  • Valérie Dufour
    • 2
    • 3
  1. 1.Psychology, School of Natural SciencesUniversity of StirlingStirlingScotland, UK
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyUniversity of St AndrewsSt AndrewsScotland, UK
  3. 3.DEPE, CNRS, IPHC, UMR 7178University of StrasbourgStrasbourgFrance

Personalised recommendations