Advertisement

Motor imagery entails task-set inhibition

  • Juliane ScheilEmail author
  • Thomas Kleinsorge
  • Baptist Liefooghe
Original Article
  • 19 Downloads

Abstract

Motor imagery requires the covert execution of a movement without any overt motor output. Previous studies indicated that motor imagery results in the prolonged inhibition of motor commands. In the present study, we investigated whether motor imagery also leads to the inhibition of more abstract task representations. To do so, we investigated the effect of motor imagery on n − 2 repetition costs, which offer an index of the extent to which task representations are inhibited. Participants switched among three tasks and among two response modes: overt and covert responding (i.e., motor imagery). N – 2 repetition costs were present when the current trial required an overt response but absent when the current trial required a covert response. Furthermore, n − 2 repetition costs were more pronounced when trial n − 1 required a covert response rather than an overt response. This pattern of results suggests that motor imagery also leads to the inhibition of abstract task representations. We discuss our findings in view of current conceptualizations of motor imagery and argue that the inhibitory mechanism entailed by motor imagery targets more than motor commands alone. Finally, we also relate our findings to the mechanisms underlying the inhibition of task representations.

Notes

Acknowledgements

The research reported in this article was supported by Grant SCHE 2004/1-1 of the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG). BL is supported by grant BOF16/MET_V/002 of Ghent University and grant G009517 N by the Flemish Government.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical standards

The present study was performed in accordance with the ethical standards laid down the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethical Committee of IfADo.

References

  1. Burianová, H., Marstaller, L., Sowman, P., Tesan, G., Rich, A. N., Williams, M., … Johnson, B. W. (2013). Multimodal functional imaging of motor imagery using a novel paradigm. NeuroImage, 71, 50–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Decety, J. (1996). The neurophysiological basis of motor imagery. Behavioural Brain Research, 77(1), 45–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Dickstein, R., & Deutsch, J. E. (2007). Motor imagery in physical therapist practice. Physical Therapy, 87(7), 942–953.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Dreher, J. C., & Berman, K. F. (2002). Fractionating the neural substrate of cognitive control processes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 99, 14595–14600.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Gade, M., & Koch, I. (2005). Linking inhibition to activation in the control of task sequences. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 530–534.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Grange, J. A., & Houghton, G. (2010). Heightened conflict in cue-target translation increases backward inhibition in set switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 1003–1009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Grange, J. A., Juvina, I., & Houghton, G. (2013). On costs and benefits of n − 2 repetitions in task switching: Towards a behavioural marker of cognitive inhibition. Psychological Research, 77, 211–222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Guillot, A., Di Rienzo, F., MacIntyre, T., Moran, A., & Collet, C. (2012). Imagining is not doing but involves specific motor commands: A review of experimental data related to motor inhibition. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Haynes, W. I., & Haber, S. N. (2013). The organization of prefrontal-subthalamic inputs in primates provides an anatomical substrate for both functional specificity and integration: Implications for basal ganglia models and deep brain stimulation. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(11), 4804–4814.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Hommel, B. (2005). How much attention does an event file need? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 1067–1082.Google Scholar
  11. Houghton, G., Pritchard, R., & Grange, J. A. (2009). The role of cue–target translation in backward inhibition of attentional set. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 466–476.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Hübner, R., & Druey, M. D. (2006). Response execution, selection, or activation: What is sufficient for response-related repetition effects under task shifting? Psychological Research, 70, 245–261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Hübner, R., & Druey, M. D. (2008). Response inhibition under task switching: Its strength depends on the amount of task-irrelevant response activation. Psychological Research, 72, 515–527.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Jeannerod, M. (1994). The representing brain. Neural correlates of motor intention and imagery. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17, 187–245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Jeannerod, M. (2001). Neural simulation of action: A unifying mechanism for motor control. NeuroImage, 14, S103–S109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Jeannerod, M. (2006). Motor cognition: What actions tell the self (No. 42). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Jost, K., Hennecke, V., & Koch, I. (2017). Task dominance determines backward inhibition in task switching. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 755.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Kiesel, A., Steinhauser, M., Wendt, M., Falkenstein, M., Jost, K., Philipp, A. M., & Koch, I. (2010). Control and interference in task switching—A review. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 849–874.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Kleinsorge, T. (1999). Response repetition benefits and costs. Acta Psychologica, 103(3), 295–310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Koch, I., Gade, M., Schuch, S., & Philipp, A. M. (2010). The role of inhibition in task switching: A review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17, 1–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Koch, I., Poljac, E., Müller, H., & Kiesel, A. (2018). Cognitive structure, flexibility, and plasticity in human multitasking—An integrative review of dual-task and task-switching research. Psychological Bulletin, 144, 557–583.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Kraeutner, S., Gionfriddo, A., Bardouille, T., & Boe, S. (2014). Motor imagery-based brain activity parallels that of motor execution: Evidence from magnetic source imaging of cortical oscillations. Brain Research, 1588, 81–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Mayr, U., & Keele, S. W. (2000). Changing internal constraints on action: The role of backward inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 4–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Nambu, A., Tokuno, H., Inase, M., & Takada, M. (1997). Corticosubthalamic input zones from forelimb representations of the dorsal and ventral divisions of the premotor cortex in the macaque monkey: Comparison with the input zones from the primary motor cortex and the supplementary motor area. Neuroscience Letters, 239(1), 13–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. O’Shea, H., & Moran, A. (2017). Does motor simulation theory explain the cognitive mechanisms underlying motor imagery? A critical review. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11, 72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Philipp, A. M., Jolicoeur, P., Falkenstein, M., & Koch, I. (2007). Response selection and response execution in task switching: Evidence from a go-signal paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 1062–1075.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Philipp, A. M., & Koch, I. (2006). Task inhibition and task repetition in task switching. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 18, 624–639.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Philipp, A. M., & Koch, I. (2010). The integration of task-set components into cognitive task representations. Psychologica Belgica, 50, 383–411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Ridderinkhof, K. R., van den Wildenberg, W. P., & Brass, M. (2014). “Don’t” versus “Won’t”: Principles, mechanisms, and intention in action inhibition. Neuropsychologia, 65, 255–262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Rieger, M., Dahm, S. F., & Koch, I. (2017). Inhibition in motor imagery: A novel action mode switching paradigm. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24(2), 459–466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Scheil, J. (2016). Effects of absolute and relative practice on n − 2 repetition costs. Acta Psychologica, 164, 65–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Scheil, J., & Kleinsorge, T. (2014). N − 2 repetition costs depend on preparation in trials n − 1 and n − 2. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40, 865–872.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Scheil, J., & Liefooghe, B. (2018). Motor command inhibition and the representation of response mode during motor imagery. Acta Psychologica, 186, 54–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Schuch, S., & Koch, I. (2003). The role of response selection for inhibition of task sets in task shifting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 92–105.Google Scholar
  35. Schuster, C., Hilfiker, R., Amft, O., Scheidhauer, A., Andrews, B., Butler, J., … Ettlin, T. (2011). Best practice for motor imagery: A systematic literature review on motor imagery training elements in five different disciplines. BMC Medline, 9, 75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Steinhauser, M., Hübner, R., & Druey, M. (2009). Adaptive control of response preparedness in task switching. Neuropsychologia, 47, 1826–1835.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Stinear, C. (2010). Prediction of recovery of motor function after stroke. The Lancet Neurology, 9(12), 1228–1232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Theeuwes, M., Liefooghe, B., De Schryver, M., & De Houwer, J. (2018). The role of motor imagery in learning via instructions. Acta Psychologica, 184, 110–123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Vandierendonck, A., Liefooghe, B., & Verbruggen, F. (2010). Task switching: interplay of reconfiguration and interference control. Psychological Bulletin, 136(4), 601–626.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Wessel, J. R., Jenkinson, N., Brittain, J. S., Voets, S. H., Aziz, T. Z., & Aron, A. R. (2016). Surprise disrupts cognition via a fronto-basal ganglia suppressive mechanism. Nature Communications, 7, 11195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  • Juliane Scheil
    • 1
    Email author
  • Thomas Kleinsorge
    • 1
  • Baptist Liefooghe
    • 2
  1. 1.Leibniz Research Centre for Working Environment and Human FactorsDortmundGermany
  2. 2.Department of Experimental-Clinical and Health PsychologyGhent UniversityGhentBelgium

Personalised recommendations