Advertisement

Dissociative associative-memory deficit as a function of primacy and recency effects

  • Jonathan GuezEmail author
  • Rotem Saar-Ashkenazy
  • Chen Tiferet-Dweck
Original Article
  • 3 Downloads

Abstract

Studies designed to explore memory for single items versus paired items (i.e., associative memory) in young adults show inconsistent results. Some studies report a decrease in associative recognition and others report mild-to-moderate or even a null effect. The studies often do not take into account stimuli serial position (SSP) when analyzing the locus of associative accuracy. Studies testing SSP often target memory for items, while studies targeting associative memory decline as a function of SSP are lacking. The objective of the current study is to test the separate and joint effect of SSP (experiments 1 + 2) and presentation duration (experiment 2) on memory recognition for items versus associations. We hypothesized that greater associative decline (compared to the expected decline in memory for items with similar serial location) will be observed for the material located at the end of a learning list than the material located at the beginning of a learning list. The results of the two experiments converged and confirmed our hypotheses; the greatest associative deficit was observed for associative material located at the end of the learning list (experiments 1 + 2) and for material presented for short durations (experiment 2). The interaction between SSP and presentation duration did not reach significance; however, a direct estimation of the cumulative deficit of SSP and presentation duration confirmed our hypothesis regarding greater associative deficit for recently presented items for short durations. These results highlight the importance of the joint and separate, effect of SSP and presentation duration to the study of associative memory decline.

Notes

Acknowledgements

This research was supported in part by the Achva Academic College grant to the first author. We are grateful to Eldana Perlman, Katya Rabinowitch, Ruslan Bernstein and Eldad Keha for their assistance in data collection and analysis.

Funding

The study was not funded.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

Dr. Guez declares no conflict of interest. Dr. Saar-Ashkenazi declares no conflict of interest. Mrs. Tiferet-Dweck declares no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

References

  1. Aggleton, J. P., & Brown, M. W. (1999). Episodic memory, amnesia, and the hippocampal–anterior thalamic axis. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(03), 425–489.‏.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Aggleton, J. P., & Shaw, C. (1996). Amnesia and recognition memory: A re-analysis of psychometric data. Neuropsychologia, 34(1), 51–62.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Baddeley, A. (1992). Working memory. Science, 255(5044), 556.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Brodie, D. A., & Murdock, B. B. (1977). Effect of presentation time on nominal and functional serial-position curves of free recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16(2), 185–200.Google Scholar
  5. Brubaker, M. S., & Naveh-Benjamin, M. (2014). The effects of presentation rate and retention interval on memory for items and associations in younger adults: A simulation of older adults’ associative memory deficit. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 21(1), 1–26.Google Scholar
  6. Capitani, E., Della Sala, S., Logie, R. H., & Spinnler, H. (1992). Recency, primacy, and memory: Reappraising and standardising the serial position curve. Cortex, 28(3), 315–342.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Corballis, M. C. (1967). Serial order in recognition and recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 74(1), 99.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Craik, F. I., & Rabinowitz, J. C. (1985). The effects of presentation rate and encoding task on age-related memory deficits. Journal of Gerontology, 40(3), 309–315.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Davachi, L., Mitchell, J. P., & Wagner, A. D. (2003). Multiple routes to memory: Distinct medial temporal lobe processes build item and source memories. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100(4), 2157–2162.Google Scholar
  10. Davachi, L., & Wagner, A. D. (2002). Hippocampal contributions to episodic encoding: insights from relational and item-based learning. Journal of Neurophysiology, 88(2), 982–990.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Diana, R. A., Yonelinas, A. P., & Ranganath, C. (2007). Imaging recollection and familiarity in the medial temporal lobe: a three-component model. Trends in cognitive sciences, 11(9), 379–386.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Eichenbaum, H., Sauvage, M., Fortin, N., Komorowski, R., & Lipton, P. (2012). Towards a functional organization of episodic memory in the medial temporal lobe. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(7), 1597–1608.Google Scholar
  13. Glanzer, M., & Cunitz, A. R. (1966). Two storage mechanisms in free recall. Journal of verbal learning and verbal behavior, 5(4), 351–360.Google Scholar
  14. Greene, R. L. (1986). Sources of recency effects in free recall. Psychological Bulletin, 99(2), 221–228.Google Scholar
  15. Guez, J., Cohen, J., Naveh-Benjamin, M., Shiber, A., Yankovsky, Y., Saar, R., & Shalev, H. (2013). Associative memory impairment in acute stress disorder: Characteristics and time course. Psychiatry Research, 209(3), 479–484.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Guez, J., & Lev, D. (2016). A picture is worth a thousand words? Not when it comes to associative memory of older adults. Psychology and Aging, 31(1), 37–41.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Guez, J., & Naveh-Benjamin, M. (2015). Proactive interference and concurrent inhibitory processes do not differentially affect item and associative recognition: Implication for the age-related associative memory deficit. Memory, 24, 1–17.Google Scholar
  18. Guez, J., Naveh-Benjamin, M., Yankovsky, Y., Cohen, J., Shiber, A., & Shalev, H. (2011). Traumatic stress is linked to a deficit in associative episodic memory. Journal of traumatic stress, 24(3), 260–267.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Henke, K., Weber, B., Kneifel, S., Wieser, H. G., & Buck, A. (1999). Human hippocampus associates information in memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 96(10), 5884–5889.Google Scholar
  20. Hirshman, E., & Hostetter, M. (2000). Using ROC curves to test models of recognition memory: The relationship between presentation duration and slope. Memory & Cognition, 28, 161–166.Google Scholar
  21. Hockey, R. (1973). Rate of presentation in running memory and direct manipulation of input-processing strategies. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 25(1), 104–111.‏.Google Scholar
  22. Isaac, C. L., & Mayes, A. R. (1999a). Rate of forgetting in amnesia: I. Recall and recognition of prose. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25(4), 942.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Isaac, C. L., & Mayes, A. R. (1999b). Rate of forgetting in amnesia: II. Recall and recognition of word lists at different levels of organization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25(4), 963.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Laughery, K. R., & Pinkus, A. L. (1966). Short-term memory: Effects of acoustic similarity, presentation rate and presentation mode. Psychonomic Science, 6(6), 285–286.Google Scholar
  25. Leding, J. K., & Lampinen, J. M. (2009). Memory conjunction errors: The effects of presentation duration and study repetition. Memory, 17(5), 597–607.‏.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Mayes, A. R., Holdstock, J. S., Isaac, C. L., Hunkin, N. M., & Roberts, N. (2002). Relative sparing of item recognition memory in a patient with adult-onset damage limited to the hippocampus. Hippocampus, 12(3), 325–340.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. McDermott, K. B., & Watson, J. M. (2001). The rise and fall of false recall: The impact of presentation duration. Journal of Memory and Language, 45(1), 160–176.Google Scholar
  28. McElree, B. (2006). Accessing recent events. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 46, 155–200.Google Scholar
  29. Murdock, B. B. Jr. (1962). The serial position effect of free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(5), 482.Google Scholar
  30. Naveh-Benjamin, M., Guez, J., Kilb, A., & Reedy, S. (2004). The associative memory deficit of older adults: Further support using face-name associations. Psychology and Aging, 19(3), 541.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Naveh-Benjamin, M., Guez, J., & Marom, M. (2003). The effects of divided attention at encoding on item and associative memory. Memory & Cognition, 31(7), 1021–1035.Google Scholar
  32. Naveh-Benjamin, M., Guez, J., & Shulman, S. (2004). Older adults’ associative deficit in episodic memory: Assessing the role of decline in attentional resources. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11(6), 1067–1073.Google Scholar
  33. Naveh-Benjamin, M., Hussain, Z., Guez, J., & Bar-On, M. (2003). Adult age differences in episodic memory: further support for an associative-deficit hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(5), 826.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Naveh-Benjamin, M., & Kilb, A. (2012). How the measurement of memory processes can affect memory performance: The case of remember/know judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(1), 194–203.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Neath, I. (1993). Distinctiveness and serial position effects in recognition. Memory & cognition, 21(5), 689–698.Google Scholar
  36. Nee, D. E., & Jonides, J. (2008). Neural correlates of access to short-term memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(37), 14228–14233.Google Scholar
  37. Nega, C. (2005). Perceptual effects and recollective experience in face recognition. Experimental Psychology, 52, 224 – 231.  https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169.52.3.224.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Old, S. R., & Naveh-Benjamin, M. (2008). Differential effects of age on item and associative measures of memory: A meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 23, 104–118.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.23.1.104.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Öztekin, I., Davachi, L., & McElree, B. (2010). Are representations in working memory distinct from representations in long-term memory? Neural evidence in support of a single store. Psychological Science, 21, 1123–1133.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  40. Ranganath, C., Yonelinas, A. P., Cohen, M. X., Dy, C. J., Tom, S. M., & D’Esposito, M. (2004). Dissociable correlates of recollection and familiarity within the medial temporal lobes. Neuropsychologia, 42(1), 2–13.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Raz, N., Lindenberger, U., Rodrigue, K. M., Kennedy, K. M., Head, D., Williamson, A., & Acker, J. D. (2005). Regional brain changes in aging healthy adults: General trends, individual differences and modifiers. Cerebral Cortex, 15(11), 1676–1689.‏.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Rubinsten, O., Anaki, D., Henik, A., Drori, S., & Faran, Y. (2005). Free association norms in the Hebrew language. In A. Henik, O. Rubinsten, & D. Anaki (Eds.) Word norms in hebrew, (pp. 17–34) Beersheba: Ben-Gurion University of the Negev.Google Scholar
  43. Rundus, D. (1971). Analysis of rehearsal processes in free recall. Journal of experimental psychology, 89(1), 63.Google Scholar
  44. Saar-Ashkenazy, R., Cohen, J. E., Guez, J., Gasho, C., Shelef, I., Friedman, A., & Shalev, H. (2014). Reduced corpus-callosum volume in posttraumatic stress disorder highlights the importance of interhemispheric connectivity for associative memory. Journal of traumatic stress, 27(1), 18–26.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Saar-Ashkenazy, R., Veksler, R., Guez, J., Jacob, Y., Shelef, I., Shalev, H., & Cohen, J. E. (2016). Breakdown of inter-hemispheric connectivity is associated with posttraumatic symptomatology and memory impairment. PLOS one, 11(2), e0144766.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  46. Sadeh, T., Ozubko, J. D., Winocur, G., & Moscovitch, M. (2016). Forgetting patterns differentiate between two forms of memory representation. Psychological Science, 27(6), 810–820.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Schon, K., Hasselmo, M. E., LoPresti, M. L., Tricarico, M. D., & Stern, C. E. (2004). Persistence of parahippocampal representation in the absence of stimulus input enhances long-term encoding: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of subsequent memory after a delayed match-to-sample task. The Journal of Neuroscience, 24(49), 11088–11097.‏.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Sperling, R., Chua, E., Cocchiarella, A., Rand-Giovannetti, E., Poldrack, R., Schacter, D. L., & Albert, M. (2003). Putting names to faces: Successful encoding of associative memories activates the anterior hippocampal formation. Neuroimage, 20(2), 1400–1410.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  49. Talmi, D., Grady, C. L., Goshen-Gottstein, Y., & Moscovitch, M. (2005). Neuroimaging the serial position curve a test of single-store versus dual-store models. Psychological Science, 16(9), 716–723.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Vargha-Khadem, F., Gadian, D. G., Watkins, K. E., Connelly, A., Van Paesschen, W., & Mishkin, M. (1997). Differential effects of early hippocampal pathology on episodic and semantic memory. Science, 277(5324), 376–380.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Waugh, N. C. (1967). Presentation time and free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 73(1), 39.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Weirich, S., Hoffmann, F., Meißner, L., Heinz, A., & Bengner, T. (2011). Sex influence on face recognition memory moderated by presentation duration and reencoding. Neuropsychology, 25(6), 806.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Wingenfeld, K., & Wolf, O. T. (2014). Stress, memory, and the hippocampus. Frontiers in Neurology and Neuroscience, 34, 109–120.Google Scholar
  54. Yonelinas, A. P. (2002). The nature of recollection and familiarity: A review of 30 years of research. Journal of memory and language, 46(3), 441–517.Google Scholar
  55. Yonelinas, A. P., Hopfinger, J. B., Buonocore, M. H., Kroll, N. E. A., & Baynes, K. (2001). Hippocampal, parahippocampal and occipitaltemporal contributions to associative and item recognition memory: An fMRI study. Neuroreport, 12(2), 359–363.PubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  • Jonathan Guez
    • 1
    • 2
    Email author
  • Rotem Saar-Ashkenazy
    • 3
    • 4
  • Chen Tiferet-Dweck
    • 3
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyAchva Academic CollegeBeer-TuviaIsrael
  2. 2.Beer-Sheva Mental Health Center, Faculty of Health SciencesBen-Gurion University of the NegevBeer-ShevaIsrael
  3. 3.Department of Cognitive Neuroscience and Zlotowski Center for NeuroscienceBen-Gurion University of the NegevBeer-ShevaIsrael
  4. 4.Department of Psychology and the Faculty of Social-WorkAshkelon Academic CollegeAshkelonIsrael

Personalised recommendations