Advertisement

Flexible coupling of covert spatial attention and motor planning based on learned spatial contingencies

  • David DignathEmail author
  • Oliver Herbort
  • Aleksandra Pieczykolan
  • Lynn Huestegge
  • Andrea Kiesel
Original Article
  • 26 Downloads

Abstract

The present study tested whether the coupling of covert attentional shifts and motor planning of pointing movements can be modulated by learning. Participants performed two tasks. As a primary movement task, they executed a pointing movement to a movement target (MT) location. As a secondary visual attention task, they identified a discrimination target (DT) that was presented shortly before initiation of the pointing movement. These DTs either occurred at the same or at different locations with the MT. A common finding in such and similar settings is the enhanced visual target identification when locations of MT and DT coincide. However, it is not known which factors govern the flexibility of spatial attention–action coupling. Here, we tested the influence of previously learned spatial contingencies between MT and DT on the coupling of covert attention and motor planning. These contingencies were manipulated in three groups (always same locations, always opposite locations, non-contingent locations) in a training session. Results indicated that in a subsequent test phase, previously learned contingencies enhanced visual identification accordingly, even when targets for the movement task and the visual task were presented at opposite sides. These results corroborate previous findings of a rather flexible interaction of attention and motor planning, and demonstrate how one can learn to control attention by means of motor planning.

Notes

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical standards

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee, and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

References

  1. Astafiev, S. V., Shulman, G. L., Stanley, C. M., Snyder, A. Z., van Essen, D. C., & Corbetta, M. (2003). Functional organization of human intraparietal and frontal cortex for attending, looking, and pointing. Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 4689–4699.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Baldauf, D., & Deubel, H. (2010). Attentional landscapes in reaching and grasping. Vision Research, 50, 999–1013.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Baldauf, D., Wolf, M., & Deubel, H. (2006). Deployment of visual attention before sequences of goal-directed hand movements. Vision Research, 46, 4355–4374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bressler, S. L., Tang, W., Sylvester, C. M., Shulman, G. L., & Corbetta, M. (2008). Top-down control of human visual cortex by frontal and parietal cortex in anticipatory visual spatial attention. Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 10056–10061.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Chun, M. M., & Jiang, Y. (1999). Top-down attentional guidance based on implicit learning of visual covariation. Psychological Science, 10, 360–365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Chun, M. M., & Jiang, Y. (2003). Implicit, long-term spatial contextual memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 224–234.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Collins, T., Schicke, T., & Röder, B. (2008). Action goal selection and motor planning can be dissociated by tool use. Cognition, 109, 363–371.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Corbetta, M. (1998). Frontoparietal cortical networks for directing attention and the eye to visual locations: Identical, independent, or overlapping neural systems?. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 95(3), 831–838.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Corbetta, M., Kincade, J. M., Ollinger, J. M., McAvoy, M. P., & Shulman, G. L. (2000). Voluntary orienting is dissociated from target detection in human posterior parietal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 292–297.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Craighero, L., Fadiga, L., Rizzolatti, G., & Umiltà, C. (1999). Action for perception: A motor-visual attentional effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1673–1692.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Deubel, H., & Schneider, W. X. (1996). Saccade target selection and object recognition: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism. Vision Research, 36, 1827–1837.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Deubel, H., & Schneider, W. X. (2003). Delayed saccades, but not delayed manual aiming movements, require visual attention shifts. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1004(1), 289–296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Deubel, H., & Schneider, W. X. (2004). Attentional selection in sequential manual movements, movements around an obstacle and in grasping. In G. W. Humphreys & M. J. Riddoch (Eds.), Attention in Action (pp. 69–91). Hove (2004): Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  14. Dore-Mazars, K., Pouget, P., & Beauvillain, C. (2004). Attentional selection during preparation of eye movements. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 69, 67–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Fagioli, S., Hommel, B., & Schubotz, R. I. (2007). Intentional control of attention: Action planning primes action-related stimulus dimensions. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 71(1), 22–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., & Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 340–347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Fecteau, J. H., Bell, A. H., & Munoz, D. P. (2004). Neural correlates of the automatic and goal-driven biases in orienting spatial attention. Journal of Neurophysiology, 92, 1728–1737.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Gherri, E., & Eimer, M. (2009). Manual response preparation disrupts spatial attention: An electrophysiological investigation of links between action and attention. Neuropsychologia, 48, 961–969.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Gottlieb, J., Balan, P. F., Oristaglio, J., & Schneider, D. (2009). Task specific computations in attentional maps. Vision Research, 49, 1216–1226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Hoffman, J. E., & Subramaniam, B. (1995). The role of visual attention in saccadic eye movements. Perception and Psychophysics, 57, 787–795.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Hoffmann, J., & Kunde, W. (1999). Location-specific target expectancies in visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1127–1141.Google Scholar
  22. Hoffmann, J., & Sebald, A. (2005). Local contextual cuing in visual search. Experimental Psychology, 52, 31–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Huestegge, L., & Adam, J. J. (2011). Oculomotor interference during manual response preparation: Evidence from the response cueing paradigm. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 73, 702–707.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Huestegge, L., & Koch, I. (2010). Fixation disengagement enhances peripheral perceptual processing: Evidence for a perceptual gap effect. Experimental Brain Research, 201, 631–640.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Huestegge, L., & Kreutzfeldt, M. (2012). Action effects in saccade control. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 19, 198–203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Huestegge, L., Pieczykolan, A., & Koch, I. (2014). Talking while looking: On the encapsulation of output system representations. Cognitive Psychology, 73, 73–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Humphreys, G. W., & Riddoch, M. J. (2005). Attention in Action: Advances from Cognitive Neuroscience. Hove: Psychology Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Jonikaitis, D., & Deubel, H. (2011). Independent allocation of attention to eye and hand targets in coordinated eye-hand movements. Psychological Science, 22(3), 339–347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Jonikaitis, D., Schubert, T., & Deubel, H. (2010). Preparing coordinated eye and hand movements: dual-task costs are not attentional. Journal of Vision, 10(14), 23–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Khan, A. Z., Blangero, A., Rossetti, Y., Salemme, R., Luaute, J., Deubel, H., & Pisella, L. (2009). Parietal damage dissociates saccade planning from presaccadic perceptual facilitation. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 383–387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Khan, A. Z., Song, J. H., & McPeek, R. M. (2011). The eye dominates in guiding attention during simultaneous eye and hand movements. Journal of Vision, 11(1), 9–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Klapetek, A., Jonikaitis, D., & Deubel, H. (2016). Attention allocation before antisaccades. Journal of Vision, 16(1), 11–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Koch, C., & Ullman, S. (1985). Shifts in selective visual attention: towards the underlying neural circuitry. Hum. Neurobiol., 4, 219–227.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Kowler, E., Anderson, E., Dosher, B., & Blaser, E. (1995). The role of attention in the programming of saccades. Vision Research, 35, 1897–1916.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Li, H. H., Barbot, A., & Carrasco, M. (2016). Saccade preparation reshapes sensory tuning. Current Biology, 26(12), 1564–1570.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Miller, J. (1988). Components of the location probability effect in visual search tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 453–471.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Moehler, T., & Fiehler, K. (2014). Effects of spatial congruency on saccade and visual discrimination performance in a dual-task paradigm. Vision Research, 105, 100–111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Montagnini, A., & Castet, E. (2007). Spatiotemporal dynamics of visual attention during saccade preparation: Independence and coupling between attention and movement planning. Journal of Vision, 7, 1–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Moore, T., & Fallah, M. (2001). Control of Eye Movements and Spatial Attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98, pp. 1273–1276.Google Scholar
  40. Musen, G. (1996). Effects of task demands on implicit memory for object-location associations. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50, 104–113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (1998). Spatial Attention: Mechanisms and Theories. In M. Sabourin, F. Craik & M. Robert (Eds.), Advances in Psychological Science: Vol.2. Biological and Cognitive Aspects (pp. 171–198). East Sussex: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  42. Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2010). Pre-motor theory of attention. Scholarpedia, 5, 6311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Rizzolatti, G., Riggio, L., Dascola, I., & Umiltá, C. (1987). Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: evidence in favor of a pre-motor theory of attention. Neuropsychologia, 25, 31–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-prime user’s guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools Inc.Google Scholar
  45. Smith, D. T., & Schenk, T. (2012). The premotor theory of attention: time to move on? Neuropsychologia, 50(6), 1104–1114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Song, J. H., & McPeek, R. M. (2009). Eye-hand coordination during target selection in a pop-out visual search. Journal of Neurophysiology, 102(5), 2681–2692.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Stewart, E. E., & Ma-Wyatt, A. (2015). The spatiotemporal characteristics of the attentional shift relative to a reach. Journal of vision, 15(5), 10–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Striemer, C., Locklin, J., Blangero, A., Rossetti, Y., Pisella, L., & Danckert, J. (2009). Attention for action? Examining the link between attention and visuomotor control deficits in a patient with optic ataxia. Neuropsychologia, 47, 1491–1499.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Thompson, K. G., & Bichot, N. P. (2005). A visual salience map in the primate frontal eye field. Progress in Brain Research, 147, 251–262.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Tipper, S. P., Lortie, C., & Baylis, G. C. (1992). Selective reaching: Evidence for action-centered attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 18, 891–891.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Wykowska, A., Schubö, A., & Hommel, B. (2009). How you move is what you see: action planning biases selection in visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(6), 1755–1769.PubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  • David Dignath
    • 1
    Email author
  • Oliver Herbort
    • 2
  • Aleksandra Pieczykolan
    • 2
    • 3
    • 4
  • Lynn Huestegge
    • 2
    • 3
  • Andrea Kiesel
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of FreiburgFreiburgGermany
  2. 2.University of WürzburgWürzburgGermany
  3. 3.Department of Psychology 3University WürzburgWürzburgGermany
  4. 4.Human Technology CenterRWTH Aachen UniversityAachenGermany

Personalised recommendations