Advertisement

Learning in the absence of overt practice: a novel (previously unseen) stimulus can trigger retrieval of an unpracticed response

  • Cai S. Longman
  • Andrea Kiesel
  • Frederick Verbruggen
Original Article
  • 14 Downloads

Abstract

Skilled performance is traditionally thought to develop via overt practice. Recent research has demonstrated that merely instructed stimulus–response (S–R) bindings can influence later performance and readily transfer across response modalities. In the present study, we extended this to include instructed category–response (C–R) associations. That is, we investigated whether merely instructed C–R bindings can trigger an unpracticed response (in a different modality) on perception of a novel (previously unseen) stimulus. In a learning-test design, participants had to classify stimuli by comparing them to perceptual category templates (Experiment 1) or semantic category descriptions (Experiment 2) presented prior to each block. During learning blocks, participants had to respond manually, respond vocally, or listen passively to the correct response being spoken. A manual response was always required at test. In test blocks, the categories could either be novel or repeated from the learning block, whereas half of the stimuli were always novel and half were always repeated from the learning block. Because stimulus and category repetitions were manipulated orthogonally, it was possible to directly compare the relative contribution of S–R and C–R associations to performance. In Experiment 1, test performance was enhanced by repeating the C–R bindings independently of the stimulus. In Experiment 2, there was also evidence of an S–R repetition benefit independent of the classification. Critically, instructed associations formed in one response modality were robust to changes in the required response, even when no overt response was required during training, indicating the need to update the traditional view of associative learning.

Notes

Funding

This work was supported by the European Research Council (Grant number 312445 awarded to Frederick Verbruggen) and by a grant of the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (KI1388/5-1, Andrea Kiesel).

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

All persons gave their informed consent prior to their inclusion in the study.

Data repository

All raw data files, R scripts (for data analysis), Matlab scripts (for data collection), and stimuli from both the experiments are stored on the Open Science Framework data repository (https://osf.io/uw2bm/).

References

  1. Brainard, D. H. (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10, 433–436.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Chein, J. M., & Schneider, W. (2012). The brain’s learning and control architecture. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(2), 78–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Cohen-Kdoshay, O., & Meiran, N. (2007). The representation of instructions in working memory leads to autonomous response activation: Evidence from the first trials in the flanker paradigm. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60(8), 1140–1154.Google Scholar
  4. Cohen-Kdoshay, O., & Meiran, N. (2009). The representation of instructions operates like a prepared reflex. Experimental Psychology, 56(2), 128–133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Dennis, I., & Perfect, T. J. (2013). Do stimulus–action associations contribute to repetition priming? Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition, 39(1), 85–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Dreisbach, G. (2012). Mechanisms of cognitive control: The functional role of task rules. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(4), 227–231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Driskell, J. E., Copper, C., & Moran, A. (1994). Does mental practice enhance performance? Journal of Applied Psychology, 79(4), 481–492.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Hazeltine, E., & Schumacher, E. H. (2016). Understanding central processes: The case against simple stimulus–response associations and for complex task representation. In B. H. Ross (Ed.), Psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 195–245). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  9. Henson, R. N., Eckstein, D., Waszak, F., Frings, C., & Horner, A. J. (2014). Stimulus-response bindings in priming. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(7), 376–384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Horner, A. J., & Henson, R. N. (2009). Bindings between stimuli and multiple response codes dominate long-lag repetition priming in speeded classification tasks. Journal of experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition, 35(3), 757–779.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Horner, A. J., & Henson, R. N. (2011). Stimulus-response bindings code both abstract and specific representations of stimuli: Evidence from a classification priming design that reverses multiple levels of response representation. Memory and Cognition, 39, 1457–1471.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Kiesel, A., Wendt, M., & Peters, A. (2007). Task switching: On the origins of response congruency effects. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 71, 117–125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Kramer, A. F., Strayer, D. L., & Buckley, J. (1990). Development and transfer of automatic processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 16(3), 505–522.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Liefooghe, B., De Houwer, J., & Wenke, D. (2013). Instruction-based response activation depends on task preparation. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 20, 481–487.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Liefooghe, B., Wenke, D., & De Houwer, J. (2012). Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition, 38(5), 1325–1335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychological Review, 95(4), 492–527.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Logan, G. D. (1990). Repetition priming and automaticity: Common underlying mechanisms? Cognitive Psychology, 22, 1–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Longman, C. S., Liefooghe, B., & Verbruggen, F. (2018). How does the (re)presentation of instructions influence their implementation? Journal of Cognition (under review).Google Scholar
  19. Longman, C. S., Milton, F., Wills, A. J., & Verbruggen, F. (2018). Transfer of learned category–response associations is modulated by instruction. Acta Psychologica, 184, 144–167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Meiran, N., Cole, W. C., & Braver, T. S. (2012). When planning results in loss of control: Intention-based reflexivity and working memory. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6(104), 1–12.Google Scholar
  21. Meiran, N., Pereg, M., Givon, E., Danieli, G., & Shahar, N. (2016). The role of working memory in rapid instructed task learning and intention-based reflexivity: An individual differences examination. Neuropsychologia, 90, 180–189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Meiran, N., Pereg, M., Kessler, Y., Cole, M. W., & Braver, T. S. (2015a). The power of instructions: Proactive configuration of stimulus–response translation. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition, 41(3), 768–786.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Meiran, N., Pereg, M., Kessler, Y., Cole, M. W., & Braver, T. S. (2015b). Reflexive activation of newly instructed stimulus–response rules: Evidence from lateralized readiness potentials in no-go trials. Cognitive Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, I15(2), 365373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Morey, R. D., Rouder, J. N., & Jamil, T. (2015). BayesFactor: Computation of Bayes factors for common designs (Version 0.9.11-1).Google Scholar
  25. Moutsopoulou, K., & Waszak, F. (2012). Across-task priming revisited: Response and task conflicts disentangled using ex-Gaussian distribution analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 38(2), 367–374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Moutsopoulou, K., & Waszak, F. (2013). Durability of classification and action learning: Differences revealed using ex-Gaussian distribution analysis. Experimental Brain Research, 226(3), 373–382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Moutsopoulou, K., Yang, Q., Desantis, A., & Waszak, F. (2015). Stimulus-classification and stimulus–action associations: Effects of repetition learning and durability. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 68(9), 1744–1757.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Pashler, H., & Baylis, G. (1991). Procedural learning: 1. Locus of practice effects in speeded choice tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition, 17(1), 20–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Pfeuffer, C. U., Hosp, T., Kimmig, E., Moutsopoulou, K., Waszak, F., & Kiesel, A. (2018). Defining stimulus representation in stimulus–response associations formed on the basis of task execution and verbal codes. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 82, 744–758.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Pfeuffer, C. U., Moutsopoulou, K., Pfister, R., Waszak, F., & Kiesel, A. (2017). Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 43(2), 328–347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Pfeuffer, C. U., Moutsopoulou, K., Waszak, F., & Kiesel, A. (2018). Multiple priming instances increase the impact of practice-based but not verbal code-based stimulus–response associations. Acta Psychologica, 184, 100–109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. R Development Core Team. (2018). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Development Core Team.Google Scholar
  33. Schmidt, J. R., De Houwer, J., & Rothermund, K. (2016). The parallel episodic processing (PEP) model 2.0: A single computational model of stimulus–response binding, contingency learning, power curves, and mixing costs. Cognitive Psychology, 91, 82–108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Schneider, W., & Shiffrin, R. F. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention. Psychological Review, 84(1), 1–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Schuster, C., Hilfiker, R., Amft, O., Scheidhauer, A., Andrews, B., Butler, J., Kischka, U., & Ettlin, T. (2011). Best practice for motor imagery: A systematic literature review on motor imagery training elements in five different disciplines. BMC Medicine, 9, 75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Strube, M. (2006). SNOOP: A program for demonstrating the consequences of premature and repeated null hypothesis testing. Behavior Research Methods, 38(1), 24–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Verbruggen, F., McLaren, R., Pereg, M., & Meiran, N. (2018). Structure and implementation of novel task rules: A cross-sectional developmental study. Psychological Science, 1:0956797618755322Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  • Cai S. Longman
    • 1
  • Andrea Kiesel
    • 2
  • Frederick Verbruggen
    • 3
  1. 1.Psychology, School of Media, Culture, and SocietyUniversity of the West of ScotlandPaisleyUK
  2. 2.University of FreiburgFreiburgGermany
  3. 3.Ghent UniversityGhentBelgium

Personalised recommendations