Advertisement

Influence of predation risk on individual spatial positioning and willingness to leave a safe refuge in a social benthic fish

  • Jesse Balaban-Feld
  • William A. Mitchell
  • Burt P. Kotler
  • Sundararaj Vijayan
  • Lotan T. Tov Elem
  • Zvika Abramsky
Original Article

Abstract

Certain individuals tend to occupy frontal positions within social groups. Less is known about how predation risk can affect individual spatial positioning and group fidelity. We tracked individuals within groups goldfish (Carassius auratus) as they left a safe, covered, microhabitat to enter a risky, open-water, microhabitat over 2 days: one with and one without an avian predator (little egret—Egretta garzetta) present. For each day, an outing index was calculated to take into account both individual order of emergence from the refuge and the proportion of outings participated in. Prior to the experiment, fish were individually marked for identification, measured, and tested for boldness to enter a novel area. Body size did not predict individual outing index scores on either control or experimental days. Boldness index scores influenced outing index scores only on days with an egret present. We found individual outing index scores to be relatively consistent across both days, regardless of the presence of a predator. However, the presence of a predator increased the mean outing index score of the highest ranked fish and decreased the number of fish that occupied lead positions, suggesting that risk amplified the behaviour of the boldest fish. Furthermore, the presence of the predator decreased individual willingness to leave the safety of cover. Thus, we show that the introduction of a predator influenced social group movement and that the boldest individuals emerge at the front of the group under risk.

Significance statement

Within social species, some bold individuals tend to occupy positions at the front of the group. Groups of goldfish were provided a safe covered refuge, and we recorded the order in which individuals left the refuge to enter an open water environment over multiple outings. We examined fish behaviour over 2 days: with and without a predator present. We found that the predator decreased the probability the fish would leave the safe refuge; however, relative individual fish behaviour and positioning were consistent over the 2 days. Interestingly, the presence of the predator amplified the behaviour of the boldest fish. Our results demonstrate that predation risk can influence group and individual prey behaviour and illustrate that bold individuals emerge at the front of groups when risk is high.

Keywords

Goldfish Social group Cohesion Predator-prey Consistency Refuge 

Notes

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by Israel Science Foundation Grant 05/14. SV is grateful to the Azrieli Foundation for the award of an Azrieli Post-doctoral Fellowship at Ben-Gurion University. The authors thank two anonymous reviewers for their insightful comments and suggestions.

Compliance with ethical standards

Ethical approval

All applicable international, national, and/or institutional guidelines for the care and use of animals were followed. The experiments were conducted in full accordance with the animal care and ethical guidelines of Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, and the Abramsky lab was granted permission to use egrets and goldfish in this study by the committee for the ethical care and use of animals in experiments (Authorization number: IL-37-07-2017).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker B, Walker S (2015) Fitting linear mixed-effects models using {lme4}. J Stat Softw 67:1–48CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Belgrad BA, Griffen BD (2016) Predator–prey interactions mediated by prey personality and predator hunting mode. Proc R Soc B 283:20160408CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Brown JS (1999) Vigilance, patch use and habitat selection: foraging under predation risk. Evol Ecol Res 1:49–71Google Scholar
  4. Brown C, Braithwaite VA (2004) Size matters: a test of boldness in eight populations of the poeciliid Brachyraphis episcopi. Anim Behav 68:1325–1329CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Brown JS, Kotler BP (2004) Hazardous duty pay and the foraging cost of predation. Ecol Lett 7:999–1014CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Brown JS, Laundré JW, Gurung M (1999) The ecology of fear: optimal foraging, game theory, and trophic interactions. J Mammal 80:385–399CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Brown C, Jones F, Braithwaite VA (2005) In situ examination of boldness–shyness traits in the tropical poeciliid, Brachyraphis episcopi. Anim Behav 70:1003–1009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Brown C, Jones F, Braithwaite VA (2007a) Correlation between boldness and body mass in natural populations of the poeciliid Brachyrhaphis episcopi. J Fish Biol 71:1590–1601CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Brown C, Burgess F, Braithwaite VA (2007b) Heritable and experiential effects on boldness in a tropical poeciliid. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 62:237–243CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Bumann D, Krause J (1993) Front individuals lead in shoals of three-spined sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus) and juvenile roach (Rutilus rutilus). Behaviour 125:189–198CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Bumann D, Krause J, Rubenstein D (1997) Mortality risk of spatial positions in animal groups: the danger of being in the front. Behaviour 134:1063–1076CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Burns AL, Herbert-Read JE, Morell LJ, Ward AJ (2012) Consistency of leadership in shoals of mosquitofish (Gambusia holbrooki) in novel and in familiar environments. PLoS One 7:e36567CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  13. Clinchy M, Sheriff MJ, Zanette LY (2013) Predator-induced stress and the ecology of fear. Funct Ecol 27:56–65CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Conradt L, Roper TJ (2000) Activity synchrony and social cohesion: a fission-fusion model. Proc R Soc Lond B 267:2213–2218CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Couzin ID, Krause J, Franks NR, Levin SA (2005) Effective leadership and decision-making in animal groups on the move. Nature 433:513–516CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Creel S, Christianson D (2008) Relationships between direct predation and risk effects. Trends Ecol Evol 23:194–201CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Cresswell W (1994) Flocking is an effective anti-predation strategy in redshanks, Tringa totanus. Anim Behav 47:433–442CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Di Bitetti MS, Janson CH (2001) Social foraging and the finder's share in capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella. Anim Behav 62:47–56CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Diaz-Uriarte R (2002) Incorrect analysis of crossover trials in animal behaviour research. Anim Behav 63:815–822CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Dowling LM, Godin J-GJ (2002) Refuge use in a killifish: influence of body size and nutritional state. Can J Zool 80:782–788CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Dunlop R, Millsopp S, Laming P (2006) Avoidance learning in goldfish (Carassius auratus) and trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss) and implications for pain perception. Appl Anim Behav Sci 97:255–271CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Fletcher K, Aebischer NJ, Baines D, Foster R, Hoodless AN (2010) Changes in breeding success and abundance of ground-nesting moorland birds in relation to the experimental deployment of legal predator control. J Appl Ecol 47:263–272CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Giardina I (2008) Collective behaviour in animal groups: theoretical models and empirical studies. HFSP J 2:205–219CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  24. Hafner H, Boy V, Gory G (1982) Feeding methods, flock size and feeding success in the little egret Egretta garzetta and the Squacco heron Ardeola ralloides in Camargue, southern France. Ardea 70:45–54Google Scholar
  25. Hamilton WD (1971) Geometry for the selfish herd. J Theor Biol 31:295–311CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Harcourt JL, Sweetman G, Johnstone RA, Manica A (2009a) Personality counts: the effect of boldness on shoal choice in three-spined sticklebacks. Anim Behav 77:1501–1505CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Harcourt JL, Ang TZ, Sweetman G, Johnstone RA, Manica A (2009b) Social feedback and the emergence of leaders and followers. Curr Biol 19:248–252CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Harris S, Ramnarine IW, Smith HG, Pettersson LB (2010) Picking personalities apart: estimating the influence of predation, sex and body size on boldness in the guppy Poecilia reticulata. Oikos 119:1711–1718CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Holopainen IJ, Tonn WM, Paszkowski CA (1997) Tales of two fish: the dichotomous biology of crucian carp (Carassius carassius (L.)) in northern Europe. Ann Zool Fenn 34:1–22Google Scholar
  30. Ihl C, Bowyer RT (2011) Leadership in mixed-sex groups of muskoxen during the snow-free season. J Mammal 92:819–827CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Ingley SJ, Rehm J, Johnson JB (2014) Size doesn't matter, sex does: a test for boldness in sister species of Brachyrhaphis fishes. Ecol Evol 4:4361–4369PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  32. Ioannou CC, Ramnarine IW, Torney CJ (2017) High-predation habitats affect the social dynamics of collective exploration in a shoaling fish. Sci Adv 3:e1602682CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  33. Johnstone RA, Manica A (2011) Evolution of personality differences in leadership. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 108:8373–8378CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Jolles JW, Boogert NJ, Sridhar VH, Couzin ID, Manica A (2017) Consistent individual differences drive collective behaviour and group functioning of schooling fish. Curr Biol 27:2862–2868CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  35. Katz MW, Abramsky Z, Kotler BP, Alteshtein O, Rosenzweig ML (2010) Playing the waiting game: predator and prey in a test environment. Evol Ecol Res 12:793–801Google Scholar
  36. Katz MW, Abramsky Z, Kotler BP, Rosenzweig ML, Alteshtein O, Vasserman G (2013) Optimal foraging of little egrets and their prey in a foraging game in a patchy environment. Am Nat 181:381–395CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Katz MW, Abramsky Z, Kotler BP, Roth I, Alteshtein O, Rosenzweig ML (2014) A predator–prey behavioural game: how does number of food patches influence foraging tactics? Evol Ecol Res 16:19–35Google Scholar
  38. Killen SS, Marras S, Steffensen JF, McKenzie DJ (2012a) Aerobic capacity influences the spatial position of individuals within fish schools. Proc R Soc Lond B 279:357–364Google Scholar
  39. Killen SS, Marras S, Ryan MR, Domenici P, McKenzie DJ (2012b) A relationship between metabolic rate and risk‐taking behaviour is revealed during hypoxia in juvenile European sea bass. Funct Ecol 26:134–143Google Scholar
  40. Killen SS, Marras S, Nadler L, Domenici P (2017) The role of physiological traits in assortment among and within fish shoals. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 372:20160233CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  41. King AJ (2010) Follow me! I’m a leader if you do; I’m a failed initiator if you don’t? Behav Process 84:671–674CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. King AJ, Johnson DD, Van Vugt M (2009) The origins and evolution of leadership. Curr Biol 19:911–916CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Krause J (1994) Differential fitness returns in relation to spatial position in groups. Biol Rev 69:187–206CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Krause J, Hoare D, Krause S, Hemelrijk CK, Rubenstein DI (2000) Leadership in fish shoals. Fish Fish 1:82–89CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Kurvers RH, Eijkelenkamp B, van Oers K, van Lith B, van Wieren SE, Ydenberg RC, Prins HH (2009) Personality differences explain leadership in barnacle geese. Anim Behav 78:447–453CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Lima SL (1995) Back to the basics of anti-predatory vigilance: the group-size effect. Anim Behav 49:11–20CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Lima SL (1998) Nonlethal effects in the ecology of predator-prey interactions. Bioscience 48:25–34CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Lusseau D, Conradt L (2009) The emergence of unshared consensus decisions in bottlenose dolphins. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 63:1067–1077CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Magurran AE (1984) Gregarious goldfish. New Sci 103:32–33Google Scholar
  50. Magurran AE, Pitcher TJ (1983) Foraging, timidity and shoal size in minnows and goldfish. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 12:147–152CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. McComb K, Shannon G, Durant SM, Sayialel K, Slotow R, Poole J, Moss C (2011) Leadership in elephants: the adaptive value of age. Proc R Soc Lond B 278:3270–3276CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Morell LJ, Romey WL (2008) Optimal individual positions within animal groups. Behav Ecol 19:909–919CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Nakayama S, Harcourt JL, Johnstone RA, Manica A (2012) Initiative, personality and leadership in pairs of foraging fish. PLoS One 7:e36606CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  54. Nakayama S, Harcourt JL, Johnstone RA, Manica A (2016) Who directs group movement? Leader effort versus follower preference in stickleback fish of different personality. Biol Lett 12:20160207CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  55. Palacios M, Warren DT, McCormick MI (2016) Sensory cues of a top-predator indirectly control a reef fish mesopredator. Oikos 125:201–209CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Pangle KL, Peacor SD, Johannsson OE (2007) Large nonlethal effects of an invasive invertebrate predator on zooplankton population growth rate. Ecology 88:402–412CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  57. Pavlov DS, Kasumyan AO (2000) Patterns and mechanisms of schooling behaviour in fish: a review. J Ichthyol 40:S163–S231Google Scholar
  58. Peterson RO, Jacobs AK, Drummer TD, Mech LD, Smith DW (2002) Leadership behaviour in relation to dominance and reproductive status in gray wolves, Canis lupus. Can J Zool 80:1405–1412CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Petit O, Bon R (2010) Decision-making processes: the case of collective movements. Behav Process 84:635–647CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Preisser EL, Bolnick DI, Benard MF (2005) Scared to death? The effects of intimidation and consumption in predator–prey interactions. Ecology 86:501–509CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. R Core Team (2016) R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria, http://www.R-project.org
  62. Reebs SG (2001) Influence of body size on leadership in shoals of golden shiners, Notemigonus crysoleucas. Behaviour 138:797–809CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Schaerf T, Herbert-Read JE, Myerscough MR, Sumpter DJ, Ward AJ (2016) Identifying differences in the rules of interaction between individuals in moving animal groups. arXiv:1601.08202Google Scholar
  64. Schuett W, Dall SR (2009) Sex differences, social context and personality in zebra finches, Taeniopygia guttata. Anim Behav 77:1041–1050CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Short KH, Petren K (2008) Boldness underlies foraging success of invasive Lepidodactylus lugubris geckos in the human landscape. Anim Behav 76:429–437CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Smith RK, Pullin AS, Stewart GB, Sutherland WJ (2010) Effectiveness of predator removal for enhancing bird populations. Conserv Biol 24:820–829CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  67. Sogard SM (1997) Size-selective mortality in the juvenile stage of teleost fishes: a review. Bull Mar Sci 60:1129–1157Google Scholar
  68. Squires VR, Daws GT (1975) Leadership and dominance relationships in merino and border Leicester sheep. Appl Anim Ethol 1:263–274CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Sueur C, Petit O (2008) Organization of group members at departure is driven by social structure in Macaca. Int J Primatol 29:1085–1098CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Voelkl B, Firth JA, Sheldon BC (2016) Nonlethal predator effects on the turn-over of wild bird flocks. Sci Rep 6:33476CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  71. Ward AJ, Hoare DJ, Couzin ID, Broom M, Krause J (2002) The effects of parasitism and body length on positioning within wild fish shoals. J Anim Ecol 71:10–14CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Ward AJ, Thomas P, Hart PJ, Krause J (2004) Correlates of boldness in three-spined sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 55:561–568CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. White JR, Meekan MG, McCormick MI, Ferrari MC (2013) A comparison of measures of boldness and their relationships to survival in young fish. PLoS One 8:e68900CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  74. Wilson AD, Godin J-GJ, Ward AJ (2010) Boldness and reproductive fitness correlates in the eastern mosquitofish, Gambusia holbrooki. Ethology 116:96–104CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Wolf M, Weissing FJ (2012) Animal personalities: consequences for ecology and evolution. Trends Ecol Evol 27:452–461CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  76. Wolf M, van Doorn GS, Leimar O, Weissing FJ (2007) Life-history trade-offs favour the evolution of animal personalities. Nature 447:581–584CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  77. Yoshida M, Nagamine M, Uematsu K (2005) Comparison of behavioural responses to a novel environment between three teleosts, bluegill Lepomis macrochirus, crucian carp Carassius langsdorfii, and goldfish Carassius auratus. Fish Sci 71:314–319CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  • Jesse Balaban-Feld
    • 1
  • William A. Mitchell
    • 2
  • Burt P. Kotler
    • 3
  • Sundararaj Vijayan
    • 1
  • Lotan T. Tov Elem
    • 1
  • Zvika Abramsky
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Life SciencesBen-Gurion UniversityBeer ShevaIsrael
  2. 2.Department of Life SciencesIndiana State UniversityTerre HauteUSA
  3. 3.The Jacob Blaustein Institute for Desert Research, Mitriani Department of Desert EcologyBen-Gurion UniversitySde BokerIsrael

Personalised recommendations