Advertisement

Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 237, Issue 11, pp 3059–3070 | Cite as

Executive working memory involved in the learning of contextual cueing effect

  • Minghui Chen
  • Chao Wang
  • Ben Sclodnick
  • Guang ZhaoEmail author
  • Xingze Liu
Research Article
  • 93 Downloads

Abstract

Implicit learning of spatial layouts occurs when target–distractor configurations repeat during a visual search task [contextual cueing; Chun and Jiang in Cogn Psychol 36(1): 28–17, 1998]. This study addressed the extent to which contextual cueing depends on executive working memory (WM). In three experiments, participants performed a contextual cueing visual search task concurrently with a WM task. The WM task was either executive (subtract 3 from each digit in WM) or non-executive (hold digits in WM), and was either low load (Experiment 1) or high load (Experiment 2). Contextual cueing was attenuated in the high-load executive WM condition. Experiment 3 replicated our findings using a within-subjects design, and confirmed the interpretation that executive functions of WM are required in contextual learning.

Keywords

Contextual cueing effect Executive working memory Storage working memory 

Notes

Acknowledgements

We thank Dr. Hong-jin Sun who provided insight and expertise that greatly assisted the research. We are immensely grateful to the anonymous reviewers for their comments. This research was supported by grants from National Natural Science Foundation of China (NSFC 31400870), China Scholarship Council (CSC) and Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC).

Author contributions

MC and CW contributed equally to this work. MC, CW, and GZ designed the experiments. MC, CW, and XL prepared the materials and performed the experiments. MC, CW, BS, GZ, and XL analyzed the data and wrote the manuscript.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Annac E, Manginelli AA, Pollmann S, Shi Z, Muller HJ, Geyer T (2013) Memory under pressure: secondary-task effects on contextual cueing of visual search. J Vis 13:6–6.  https://doi.org/10.1167/13.13.6 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Awh E, Belopolsky AV, Theeuwes J (2012) Top-down versus bottom-up attentional control: a failed theoretical dichotomy. Trends Cogn Sci 16(8):437–443.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.06.010 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  3. Baddeley A (1992) Working memory. Science 255(5044):556–559.  https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1736359 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Baddeley A (1996) Exploring the central executive. Q J Exp Psychol A Human Exp Psychol 49A(1):5–28.  https://doi.org/10.1080/027249896392784 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Baddeley AD (1997) Human memory: theory and practice. Psychology PressGoogle Scholar
  6. Baddeley A (2012) Working memory: theories, models, and controversies. Annu Rev Psychol 63(1):1–29.  https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100422 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Baddeley AD, Hitch G (1974) Working memory. In: Bower GH (ed) Psychology of learning and motivation, vol 8, pp 47–89.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60452-1 Google Scholar
  8. Baddeley A, Gathercole SE, Papagno C (1998) The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychol Rev 105(1):158–173.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.105.1.158 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Chun MM, Jiang Y (1998) Contextual cueing: implicit learning and memory of visual context guides spatial attention. Cogn Psychol 36(1):28–71.  https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1998.0681 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Chun MM, Jiang Y (2003) Implicit, long-term spatial contextual memory. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 29(2):224–234.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.29.2.224 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Cowan N (2001) The magical number 4 in short-term memory: a reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behav Brain Sci 24(1):87–114.  https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X01003922 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Cowan N (2010) The magical mystery four: how is working memory capacity limited, and why? Curr Dir Psychol Sci 19(1):51–57.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721409359277 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  13. D’Esposito M, Postle BR, Ballard D, Lease J (1999) Maintenance versus manipulation of information held in working memory: an Event-Related fMRI Study. Brain Cogn 41(1):66–86.  https://doi.org/10.1006/brcg.1999.1096 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Han S-H, Kim M-S (2004) Visual search does not remain efficient when executive working memory is working. Psychol Sci 15(9):623–628CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Janacsek K, Nemeth D (2013) Implicit sequence learning and working memory: correlated or complicated? Cortex 49(8):2001–2006.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2013.02.012 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Jiang Y, Leung AW (2005) Implicit learning of ignored visual context. Psychon Bull Rev 12(1):100–106.  https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196353 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. LaRocque JJ, Lewis-Peacock JA, Postle BR (2014) Multiple neural states of representation in short-term memory? It’s a matter of attention. Front Human Neurosci.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Lee K-M, Kang S-Y (2002) Arithmetic operation and working memory: differential suppression in dual tasks. Cognition 83(3):B63–B68.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00010-0 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Luck SJ, Vogel EK (1997) The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions. Nature 390:3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Manginelli AA, Geringswald F, Pollmann S (2012) Visual search facilitation in repeated displays depends on visuospatial working memory. Exp Psychol 59(1):47–54.  https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000125 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Manginelli AA, Langer N, Klose D, Pollmann S (2013) Contextual cueing under working memory load: selective interference of visuospatial load with expression of learning. Atten Percept Psychophys 75(6):1103–1117.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-013-0466-5 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Martini M, Furtner MR, Sachse P (2013) Working memory and its relation to deterministic sequence learning. PLoS One 8(2):e56166.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0056166 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  23. Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD (2000) The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn Psychol 41(1):49–100.  https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Morey RD (2008) Confidence Intervals from Normalized Data: A correction to Cousineau (2005). Tutorials Quant Methods Psychol 42(2):61–64.  https://doi.org/10.20982/tqmp.04.2.p061 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Oh S-H, Kim M-S (2004) The role of spatial working memory in visual search efficiency. Psychon Bull Rev 11:275–281.  https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03196570 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Soto D, Rotshtein P, Kanai R (2014) Parietal structure and function explain human variation in working memory biases of visual attention. NeuroImage 89:289–296.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.036 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. St Clair-Thompson HL, Gathercole SE (2006) Executive functions and achievements in school: shifting, updating, inhibition, and working memory. Q J Exp Psychol 59(4):745–759.  https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210500162854 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Travis SL, Mattingley JB, Dux PE (2012) On the role of working memory in spatial contextual cueing. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 39(1):208–219.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028644 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Travis SL, Mattingley JB, Dux PE (2013) On the role of working memory in spatial contextual cueing. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 39(1):208–219.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028644 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Tuholski SW, Engle RW, Baylis GC (2001) Individual differences in working memory capacity and enumeration. Mem Cogn 29(3):484–492.  https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196399 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Vickery TJ, Sussman RS, Jiang YV (2010) Spatial context learning survives interference from working memory load. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 36(6):1358–1371.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020558 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  32. Wang T, Ren X, Schweizer K (2017) Learning and retrieval processes predict fluid intelligence over and above working memory. Intelligence 61:29–36.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2016.12.005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Zellin M, von Mühlenen A, Müller HJ, Conci M (2014) Long-term adaptation to change in implicit contextual learning. Psychon Bull Rev 21(4):1073–1079.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-013-0568-z CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Faculty of PsychologyTianjin Normal UniversityTianjinChina
  2. 2.Department of Psychology, Neuroscience and BehaviourMcMaster UniversityHamiltonCanada
  3. 3.Research Center of Brain and Cognitive ScienceLiaoning Normal UniversityDalianChina
  4. 4.Medical Psychological CenterSecond Xiangya Hospital of Central South UniversityChangshaChina

Personalised recommendations