Advertisement

Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 237, Issue 6, pp 1421–1430 | Cite as

Emergence of anticipatory actions in a novel task

  • Oliver HerbortEmail author
  • Wilfried Kunde
Research Article

Abstract

Humans normally adapt earlier segments of multistep motor actions in such a way that the execution of later segments is facilitated. For example, the kinematics of grasping movements are adapted to the requirements of the intended subsequent object manipulations. Here we studied which factors foster adaptation of earlier action segments to later ones in a novel task for which no prior experience existed. Participants executed a two-step isometric force production task, in which the force produced in the first segment determined the difficulty of the second segment. Adaptation of the first segment to the second one benefited from explicit knowledge of the dependency between both segments but not from extensive prior experience with the second segment. These observations show that adaptation of motor actions to subsequent actions demands the construction of a task representation that allows to plan the first action segment with respect to its successor. How specifically the first segment is tailored to the second one does not depend on prior experience with the second segment but depends on experience from performing the interdependent two-step action sequence.

Keywords

Motor control Action planning Anticipatory action Action sequences Isometric force 

Notes

Acknowledgements

We thank Georg Schüssler and Albrecht Sebald for technical support and Lorena Fleischmann for preparing and running the experiment.

Supplementary material

Supplementary material 1 (MP4 3982 KB)

References

  1. Bolstad G, Ersland A (1978) Energy metabolism in different human skeletal muscles during voluntary isometric contractions. Eur J Appl Physiol Occup Physiol 38:171–179.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00430075 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Cohen RG, Rosenbaum DA (2004) Where grasps are made reveals how grasps are planned: generation and recall of motor plans. Exp Brain Res 157:486–495.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-004-1862-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Cowie D, Smith L, Braddick O (2010) The development of locomotor planning for end-state comfort. Perception 39(5):661–670.  https://doi.org/10.1068/p6343 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Fitts PM (1962) Factors in complex skill training. In: Glaser R (ed) Training research and education. University of Pittsburgh Press, Pittsburgh, pp 177–197Google Scholar
  5. Fitts PM (1964) Perceptual-motor skill learning. In: Melton AW (ed) Categories of human learning. Academic Press, Oxford, pp 243–285CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Fuelscher I, Williams J, Wilmut K, Enticott PG, Hyde C (2016) Modeling the maturation of grip selection planning and action representation: insights from typical and atypical motor development. Front Psychol 7:108.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00108 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Herbort O (2012) Where to grasp a tool? Task-dependent adjustments of tool transformations by tool users. Z Psychol 220:37–43.  https://doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604/a000089 Google Scholar
  8. Herbort O, Butz MV (2012) The continuous end-state comfort effect: weighted integration of multiple biases. Psychol Res 76:345–363.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0334-7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Herbort O, Butz MV, Kunde W (2014) The contribution of cognitive, kinematic, and dynamic factors to anticipatory grasp selection. Exp Brain Res 232:1677–1688.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-014-3849-5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Herbort O, Mathew H, Kunde W (2017) Habit outweighs planning in grasp selection for object manipulation. Cogn Psychol 92:127–140.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2016.11.008 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Herbort O, Kirsch W, Kunde W (2018) Grasp planning for object manipulation without simulation of the object manipulation action. J Exp Psychol 45(2):237–254Google Scholar
  12. Huhn JM, Potts CA, Rosenbaum DA (2016) Cognitive framing in action. Cognition 151:42–51.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.02.015 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Jovanovic B, Schwarzer G (2011) Learning to grasp efficiently: the development of motor planning and the role of observational learning. Vis Res 51:945–954.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2010.12.00 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Mathew H, Kunde W, Herbort O (2017) Inverting the planning gradient: adjustment of grasps to late segments of multi-step object manipulations. Exp Brain Res 235:1397–1409.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-017-4892-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Newell KM, Carlton LG (1988) Force variability in isometric responses. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 14:37–44.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.14.1.37 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Rieger M (2012) Motor imagery in typing: effects of typing style and action familiarity. Psychonom Bull Rev 19:101–107.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-011-0178-6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Rosenbaum DA, Marchak F, Barnes HJ, Vaughan J, Slotta JD, Jorgensen MJ (1990) Constraints for action selection: overhand versus underhand grips. In: Jeannerod M (ed) Attention and performance, vol XIII. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, pp 321–345Google Scholar
  18. Rosenbaum DA, Chapman KM, Weigelt M, Weiss DJ, van der Wel R (2012) Cognition, action, and object manipulations. Psychol Null 138:924–946.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027839 Google Scholar
  19. Schmidt RA, Zelaznik H, Hawkins B, Frank JS, Quinn JT (1979) Motor-output variability: a theory for the accuracy of rapid motor acts. Psychol Rev 86:415–451.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.86.5.415 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Short MW, Cauraugh JH (1999) Precision hypothesis and the end-state comfort effect. Acta Psychol 100(3):243–252.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0001-6918(98)00020-1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Sosnoff JJ, Newell KM (2005) Intermittent visual information and the multiple time scales of visual motor control of continuous isometric force production. Perc Psychophys 67:335–344.  https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03206496 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Stöckel T, Hughes CML, Schack T (2012) Representation of postures and anticipatory motor planning in children. Psychol Res 76:768–776.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0387-7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Toussaint L, Tahej P-K, Thibaut J-P, Possamai C-A, Badets A (2013) On the link between action planning and motor imagery: a developmental study. Exp Brain Res 231:331–339.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-013-3698-7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Willingham DB (1998) A neuropsychological theory of motor skill learning. Psychol Rev 105:558–584.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.105.3.558 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Wing AM, Flanagan JR, Richardson J (1997) Anticipatory postural adjustments in stance and grip. Exp Brain Res 116:122–130.  https://doi.org/10.1007/PL00005732 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Wunsch K, Henning A, Aschersleben G, Weigelt M (2013) A systematic review of the end-state comfort effect in normally developing children and in children with developmental disorders. J Motor Learn Develop 1:59–76.  https://doi.org/10.1123/jmld.1.3.59 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Wunsch K, Pfister R, Henning A, Aschersleben G, Weigelt M (2016) No interrelation of motor planning and executive functions across young ages. Front Psychol 7:1031.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01031 Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of WürzburgWürzburgGermany

Personalised recommendations