Advertisement

Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 237, Issue 2, pp 557–571 | Cite as

Where is my mind? Examining mind-wandering and vigilance performance

  • Alexis R. NeigelEmail author
  • Victoria L. Claypoole
  • Nicholas W. Fraulini
  • Grace E. Waldfogle
  • James L. Szalma
Research Article
  • 50 Downloads

Abstract

Vigilance is the ability to sustain attention to information for prolonged periods of time, particularly in environments where critical signals may be rare. Recent research in the domain of mind-wandering has suggested that processes associated with mind-wandering may underpin the typical decline in vigilance task performance. Current methods for measuring mind-wandering either disrupt vigils by asking probe questions throughout the task, or, require observers to reflect on how much mind-wandering occurred during the task upon conclusion of the vigil. Across three experimental studies, we treat mind-wandering as an individual difference, which was measured pre- and post-vigil. We argue this technique is a more holistic representation of mind-wandering and is less intrusive than probe measures, which serve to disrupt the vigil. The results of our first experiment challenge previous results in the literature: higher rates of mind-wandering were associated with improved correct detection performance. Interestingly, the second experiment suggests that increases in mind-wandering were not linked to vigilance performance deficits. However, significant differences in global workload emerged in the second experiment, implying individuals low in mind-wandering report greater workload. In a third experiment, wherein we manipulated event rate, mind-wandering typology had no significant effect on vigilance performance. We conclude with a discussion of the relevance of individual differences in mind-wandering in vigilance research considering the present findings.

Keywords

Attention Human performance Mind-wandering Sustained attention Vigilance 

Notes

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by the University of Central Florida Dean’s Dissertation Completion Fellowship, which was awarded to Dr. Alexis R. Neigel in 2017 and Dr. Victoria L. Claypoole in 2018.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The views and conclusions contained in this document are those of the authors and should not be interpreted as representing the official policies, either expressed or implied, of the US Air Force Academy or the US Government. The US Government is authorized to reproduce and distribute reprints for Government purposes notwithstanding any copyright notation herein.

References

  1. Caggiano DM, Parasuraman R (2004) The role of memory representation in the vigilance decrement. Psychon Bull Rev 11:932–937CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Claypoole VL, Szalma JL (2018) Independent coactors may improve performance and lower workload: viewing vigilance under social facilitation. Hum Factors 60:822–832CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Claypoole VL, Dever DA, Denues KL, Szalma JL (2018) The effects of event rate on cognitive based vigilance tasks. Hum Factors 2018:1–11Google Scholar
  4. Epling SL, Russell PN, Helton WS (2016) A new semantic vigilance task: vigilance decrement, workload, and sensitivity to dual-task costs. Exp Brain Res 234:133–139.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-015-4444-0 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Finomore V, Matthews G, Shaw T et al (2009) Predicting vigilance: a fresh look at an old problem. Ergo 52:791–808CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Finomore VS, Shaw TH, Warm JS, Matthews G, Boles DB (2013) Viewing the workload of vigilance through the lenses of the NASA-TLX and the MRQ. Hum Factors 55:1044–1063CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Fraulini NW, Hancock GM, Neigel AR, Claypoole VL, Szalma JL (2017) A critical examination of the research and theoretical underpinnings discussed in Thomson, Besner, and Smilek (2016): a commentary. Psychol Rev 124:525–531CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Golchert J, Smallwood J, Jefferie E et al (2017) Individual variation in intentionality in the mind wandering state is reflected in the integration of the default-mode, fronto-parietal, and limbic networks. Neuroimage 146:226–235CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Grier RA (2015) How high is high? A meta-analysis of NASA-TLX global workload scores. Proc Hum Factors Ergon Soc 59:1727–1731CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Grier RA, Warm JS, Dember WN, Matthews G, Galinsky TL, Parasuraman R (2003) The vigilance decrement reflects limitations in effortful attention, not mindlessness. Hum Factors 45:349–359CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Guralnick MJ (1973) Effects of event rate and signal difficulty on observing responses and detection measures in vigilance. J Exp Psychol 99:261CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Hancock PA (1996) Effects of control order, augmented feedback, input device and practice on tracking performance and perceived workload. Ergonomics 39:1146–1162CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Hancock PA (2017) On the nature of vigilance. Hum Factors 59:35–43CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Hancock PA, Warm JS (1989) A dynamic model of stress and sustained attention. Hum Factors 31:519–537CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Hancock PA, Sawyer BD, Stafford S (2015) The effects of display size on performance. Ergonomics 58:337–354CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Hart SG, Staveland LE (1988) Development of NASA-TLX (Task Load Index): results of empirical and theoretical research. Adv Psychol 52:139–183CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Head J, Helton WS (2014) Practice does not make perfect in a modified sustained attention to response task. Exp Brain Res 232:565–573CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Helton WS, Russell PN (2011) Feature absence–presence and two theories of lapses of sustained attention. Psychol Res 75:384–392CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Helton WS, Russell PN (2012) Brief mental breaks and content-free cues may not keep you focused. Exp Brain Res 219:37–46CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Helton WS, Russell PN (2013) Visuospatial and verbal working memory load: effects on visuospatial vigilance. Exp Brain Res 224:429–436CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Helton WS, Warm JS (2008) Signal salience and the mindlessness theory of vigilance. Acta Psychol 129:18–25.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.04.002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Hirst W, Kalmar D (1987) Characterizing attentional resources. J Exp Psychol Gen 116:68–81CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Jerison HJ, Pickett RM (1964) Vigilance: the importance of the elicited observing rate. Science 143:970–971CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Kahneman D (1973) Attention and effort. Prentice Hall, New JerseyGoogle Scholar
  25. Koelega HS, Brinkman J, Hendriks L, Verbaten MN (1989) Processing demands, effort, and individual differences in four different vigilance tasks. Hum Factors 31:45–62CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Mackworth NH (1948) The breakdown of vigilance during prolonged visual search. Q J Exp Psychol 1:6–21CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Matthews G (2016) Multidimensional profiling of task stress states for human factors: a brief review. Hum Factors 58:801–813.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0018720816653688 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Matthews G, Davies DR (1998) Arousal and vigilance: still vital at fifty. Proc Hum Factors Ergon Soc 42:772–776CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Matthews G, Davies DR, Holley PJ (1993) Cognitive predictors of vigilance. Hum Factors 35:3–24CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Matthews G, Campbell SE, Falconer S et al (2002) Fundamental dimensions of subjective state in performance settings: task engagement, distress, and worry. Emotion 2:315–340CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Matthews G, Warm JS, Shaw TH et al (2014) Predicting battlefield vigilance: a multivariate approach to assessment of attentional resources. Ergo 57:856–875CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. McVay JC, Kane MJ (2012) Drifting from slow to “d’oh!”: working memory capacity and mind wandering predict extreme reaction times and executive control errors. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 38:525–549CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Meuter RF, Lacherez PF (2016) When and why threats go undetected: impacts of event rate and shift length on threat detection accuracy during airport baggage screening. Hum Factors 58:218–228CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Moray N (1967) Where is capacity limited? A survey and a model. Acta Psychol 27:84–92CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Mouloua M, Parasuraman R (1995) Aging and cognitive vigilance: effects of spatial uncertainty and event rate. Exp Aging Res 21:17–32CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Navon D, Gopher D (1979) On the economy of the human information processing system. Psychol Rev 86:214–255CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Neigel AR, Claypoole VL, Hancock GM, Fraulini NW, Szalma JL (2018) Ceci n’est pas un walrus: lexical processing in vigilance performance. Exp Brain Res 236:867–879CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Norman DA, Bobrow DG (1975) On data-limited and resource-limited processes. Cogn Psychol 7:44–64CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Parasuraman R (1979) Memory load and event rate control sensitivity decrements in sustained attention. Science 205:924–927CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Parasuraman R, Davies DR (1977) A taxonomic analysis of vigilance performance. In: Vigilance. Springer, Boston, pp 559–574CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Parasuraman R, de Visser E, Clarke E, McGarry WR, Hussey E, Shaw T, Thompson JC (2009) Detecting threat-related intentional actions of others: effects of image quality, response mode, and target cuing on vigilance. J Exp Psychol App 15:275–290CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Robertson IH, Manly T, Andrade J, Baddeley BT, Yiend J (1997) ‘Oops!’: performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects. Neuropsychol 35:747–758CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. See JE, Howe SR, Warm JS, Dember WN (1995) Meta-analysis of the sensitivity decrement in vigilance. Psychol Bull 117:230–249CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Seli P, Carriere JSA, Smilek D (2014) Not all mind wandering is created equal: dissociating deliberate from spontaneous mind wandering. Psychol Res 79:750–758CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Seli P, Jonker TR, Cheyne JA, Cortes K, Smilek D (2015a) Can research observers comment authoritatively on the validity of their self-reports of mind wandering and task engagement? J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 41:703–709CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Seli P, Cheyne JA, Xu M, Purdon C, Smilek D (2015b) Motivation, intentionality, and mind-wandering: implications for assessments of task-unrelated thought. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 41:1417–1425CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Seli P, Risko EF, Smilek D, Schacter DL (2016a) Mind-wandering with and without intention. Trends Cogn Sci 20:605–617CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Seli P, Risko EF, Smilek D (2016b) Assessing the associations among trait and state levels of deliberate and spontaneous mind wandering. Conscious Cogn 41:50–56CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Seli P, Carriere JSA, Smilek D (2017) Not all mind wandering is created equal: dissociating deliberate from spontaneous mind wandering. Psychol Res 79:750–758CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Seli P, Smilek D, Ralph BCW, Schacter DL (2018) The awakening of the attention: evidence for a link between the monitoring of mind wandering and prospective goals. J Exp Psychol Gen 147:431–443CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Shaw TH, Matthews G, Warm JS et al (2010) Individual differences in vigilance: personality, ability and states of stress. J Res Pers 44:297–308CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Smit AS, Eling PA, Coenen AM (2004) Mental effort causes vigilance decrease due to resource depletion. Acta Psychol 115:35–42CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Szalma JL, Teo GWL (2012) Spatial and temporal task characteristics as stress: a test of the dynamic adaptability theory of stress, workload, and performance. Acta Psychol 139:471–485.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2011.12.009 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Thomson DR, Besner D, Smilek D (2013) In pursuit of off-task thought: mind wandering-performance trade-offs while reading aloud and color naming. Front Psychol 4:1–11CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Thomson DR, Seli P, Besner D, Smilek D (2014) On the link between mind wandering and task performance over time. Conscious Cogn 27:14–26CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Thomson DR, Besner D, Smilek D (2015) A resource-control account of sustained attention: evidence from mind-wandering and vigilance paradigms. Perspect Psychol Sci 10:82–96CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Warm JS, Dember WN (1998) Tests of vigilance taxonomy. In: Hoffman RB, Sherrick MF, Warm JS (eds) Viewing psychology as a whole: the integrative science of William N. Dember. American Psychological Association, Washington, DCGoogle Scholar
  58. Warm JS, Jerison HJ (1984) The psychophysics of vigilance. In: Warm JS (ed) Sustained attention in human performance. Wiley, Chichester, pp 15–59Google Scholar
  59. Warm JS, Dember WN, Hancock PA (1996) Vigilance and workload in automated systems. In: Parasuraman R, Mouloua M (eds) Human factors in transportation. Automation and human performance: theory and applications. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc., New Jersey, pp 183–200Google Scholar
  60. Warm JS, Parasuraman R, Matthews G (2008) Vigilance requires hard mental work and is stressful. Hum Factors 50:433–441CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Yeh YY, Wickens CD (1988) Dissociation of performance and subjective measures of workload. Hum Factors 30:111–120CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© This is a U.S. government work and its text is not subject to copyright protection in the United States; however, its text may be subject to foreign copyright protection 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  • Alexis R. Neigel
    • 1
    • 2
    Email author
  • Victoria L. Claypoole
    • 3
  • Nicholas W. Fraulini
    • 3
  • Grace E. Waldfogle
    • 3
  • James L. Szalma
    • 3
  1. 1.Department of the Behavioral and Leadership SciencesUnited States Air Force AcademyColorado SpringsUSA
  2. 2.Warfighter Effectiveness Research Center (WERC), United States Air Force AcademyColorado SpringsUSA
  3. 3.Department of PsychologyUniversity of Central Florida, College of SciencesOrlandoUSA

Personalised recommendations