Advertisement

Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 237, Issue 2, pp 417–426 | Cite as

Mirror-hand selection is influenced by training perspective and model skill level in a motor-learning task

  • John J. BuchananEmail author
Research Article
  • 57 Downloads

Abstract

This study examined mirror and non-mirror arm selection processes in an observational learning context. Observer groups watched either a novice (instruction or discovery) or skilled model performing a bimanual task with the right arm leading the left arm. The models were viewed from a third-person perspective. Observers of the skilled model more often selected a mirror-image (left-hand) hand-lead in post-observations tests, while observers of the novice models more often selected a non-mirror image (right hand) hand-lead in post-observation tests. This is a novel finding regarding arm selection processes in a learning context, yet it is consistent with imaging data that has revealed specific neural areas linked to the selection of mirror and non-mirror imitation processes for first- and third-person viewing perspectives. The skilled model also supported more accurate and stable performance of the bimanual task in observers compared to the instruction and novice models. It is concluded that a skilled model supports attention focus being directed at pattern analysis, while novice models support attention focus being allocated to strategy identification first, followed by pattern analysis.

Keywords

Bimanual Lateralization Coordination dynamics Perception–action Instructions Relative phase 

Notes

Acknowledgements

The author would like to thank an anonymous reviewer for raising the issue of generalization and the encoding of the spatial and temporal characteristics of the bimanual task by the AON.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Andrieux M, Proteau L (2013) Observation learning of a motor task: who and when? Exp Brain Res 229:125–137.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-013-3598-x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Andrieux M, Proteau L (2014) Mixed observation favors motor learning through better estimation of the model’s performance. Exp Brain Res 232:3121–3132.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-014-4000-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Anquetil T, Jeannerod M (2007) Simulated actions in the first and in the third person perspectives share common representations. Brain Res 1130:125–129CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bardi L, Bundt C, Notebaert W, Brass M (2015) Eliminating mirror responses by instructions. Cortex 70:128–136.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2015.04.018 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bianchi I, Savardi U, Burro R, Martelli MF (2014) Doing the opposite to what another person is doing. Acta Physiol (Oxf) 151:117–133.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.06.003 Google Scholar
  6. Bird G, Heyes C (2005) Effector-dependent learning by observation of a finger movement sequence. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 31:262–275CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Blandin Y, Proteau L (2000) On the cognitive basis of observational learning: development of mechanisms for the detection and correction of errors. Q J Exp Psychol 53A:846–867CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Blandin Y, Lhuisset L, Proteau L (1999) Cognitive processes underlying observational learning of motor skills. Q J Exp Psychol 52A:957–979CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Buchanan JJ, Dean N (2014) Consistently modeling the same movement strategy is more important than model skill level in observational learning contexts. Acta Physiol (Oxf) 146:19–27Google Scholar
  10. Buchanan JJ, Ryu YU (2005) The interaction of tactile information and movement amplitude in a multijoint bimanual circle-tracing task: phase transitions and loss of stability. Q J Exp Psychol 58A:769–787CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Byblow WD, Carson RG, Goodman D (1994) Expressions of asymmetries and anchoring in bimanual coordination. Hum Mov Sci 13:3–28CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Calvo-Merino B, Glaser DE, Grezes RE, Passingham RE, Haggard P (2005) Action observation and acquired motor skills: an fMRI study with expert dancers. Cereb Cortex 15:1243–1249CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Carson RG, Kelso JAS (2004) Governing coordination: behavioural principles and neural correlates. Exp Brain Res 154:267–274CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Carson RG, Thomas J, Summers JJ, Walters MR, Semjen A (1997) The dynamics of bimanual circle drawing. Q J Exp Psychol 50A:664–683CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Cattaneo L, Rizzolatti G (2009) The mirror neuron system. Neurol Rev 66:557–560Google Scholar
  16. Cook R, Bird G, Catmur C, Press C, Heyes C (2014) Mirror neurons: from origin to function. Behav Brain Sci 37:177–192.  https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x13000903 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Cross ES, Hamilton A, Grafton ST (2006) Building a motor simulation de novo: Observation of dance by dancers. Neuroimage 31:1257–1267.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.033 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Franz EA, Ford S, Werner S (2007) Brain and cognitive processes of imitation in bimanual situations: Making inferences about mirror neuron systems. Brain Res 1145:138–149.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2007.01.136 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Freundlieb M, Kovacs AM, Sebanz N (2016) When do humans spontaneously adopt another’s visuospatial perspective? J Exp Psychol-Hum Percept Perform 42:401–412.  https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000153 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Garland TB, Sanchez CA (2013) Rotational perspective and learning procedural tasks from dynamic media. Comput Educ 69:31–37.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2013.06.014 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Harris DJ, Vine SJ, Wilson MR, McGrath JS, LeBel ME, Buckingham G (2017) The effect of observing novice and expert performance on acquisition of surgical skills on a robotic platform. Plos One 12  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0188233
  22. Hayes SJ, Ashford D, Bennett SJ (2008) Goal-directed imitation: the means to an end. Acta Physiol (Oxf) 127:407–415.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2007.07.009 Google Scholar
  23. Iacoboni M, Woods RP, Brass M, Bekkering H, Mazziotta JC, Rizzolatti G (1999) Cortical Mechanisms of human imitation. Science 286:2526–2528CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Kelso JAS, Scholz JP, Schoner G (1986) Nonequilibrium phase-transitions in coordinated biological motion—critical fluctuations. Phys Lett A 118:279–284.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0375-9601(86)90359-2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Koehler S, Egetemeir J, Stenneken P, Koch SP, Pauli P, Fallgatter AJ, Herrmann MJ (2012) The human execution/observation matching system investigated with a complex everyday task: a functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) study. Neurosci Lett 508:73–77.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2011.12.021 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Maeda F, Kleiner-Fisman G, Pascual-Leone A (2002) Motor facilitation while observing hand actions: specificity of the effect and role of observer’s orientation. J Neurophysiol 87:1329–1335.  https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00773.2000 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Martens R, Burwitz L, Zuckerman J (1976) Modeling effects on motor performance. Res Q 47:277–291Google Scholar
  28. McCullagh P, Caird JK (1990) Correct and learning-models and the use of model knowledge of results in the acquisition and retention of a motor skill. J Hum Mov Stud 18:107–116Google Scholar
  29. Press C, Ray E, Heyes C (2009) Imitation of lateralised body movements: Doing it the hard way. Laterality 14:515–527.  https://doi.org/10.1080/13576500802607990 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Rizzolatti G, Craighero L (2004) The mirror-neuron system. Annu Rev Neurosci 27:169–192CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Rohbanfard H, Proteau L (2011a) Effects of the model’s handedness and observer’s viewpoint on observational learning. Exp Brain Res 214:567–576.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2856-z CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Rohbanfard H, Proteau L (2011b) Learning through observation: a combination of expert and novice models favors learning. Exp Brain Res 215:183–197CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Salesse R, Oullier O, Temprado JJ (2005) Plane of motion mediates the coalition of constraints in rhythmic bimanual coordination. J Mot Behav 37:454–464CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Sebanz N, Knoblich G, Prinz W (2003) Representing others’ actions: just like one’s own? Cognition 88:B11–B21.  https://doi.org/10.1016/s0010-0277(03)00043-x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Shmuelof L, Zohary E (2006) A mirror representation of others’ actions in the human anterior parietal cortex. J Neurosci 26:9736–9742.  https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.1836-06.2006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Shmuelof L, Zohary E (2008) Mirror-image representation of action in the anterior parietal cortex. Nat Neurosci 11:1267–1269.  https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2196 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Stevens JA (2005) Interference effects demonstrate distinct roles for visual and motor imagery during the mental representation of human action. Cognition 95:329–350.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2004.02.008 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Summers JJ, Semjen A, Carson RG, Thomas J, Glencross DJ, Piek JP (1995) Going around in circles: the dynamics of bimanual circling. In: Motor control and sensory motor integration: Issues and directions. Elsevier Science, New York, pp 231–252CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Swinnen SP (2002) Intermanual coordination: from behavioral principles to neural-network interactions. Nat Rev 3:350–361CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Temprado JJ, Swinnen SP, Coutton-Jean C, Salesse R (2007) Symmetry constraints mediate the learning and transfer of bimanual coordination patterns across planes of motion. J Mot Behav 39:115–125.  https://doi.org/10.3200/jmbr.39.2.115-125 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Weir PL, Leavitt JL (1990) The effects of model’s skill level and model’s knowledge of results on the acquisition of an aiming task. Hum Mov Sci 9:369–383CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Wilson AD, Snapp-Childs W, Bingham GP (2010) Perceptual learning immediately yields new stable motor coordination. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 36:1508–1514.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020412 CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Perception-Action Dynamics Lab, Department of Health and KinesiologyTexas A&M UniversityCollege StationUSA

Personalised recommendations