Advertisement

Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 236, Issue 10, pp 2781–2796 | Cite as

The oddball effect on P3 disappears when feature relevance or feature-response mappings are unknown

  • Rolf Verleger
  • Magdalena Keppeler
  • Jona Sassenhagen
  • Kamila Śmigasiewicz
Research Article

Abstract

The P3b component of human event-related EEG potentials is larger with rare than frequent task-relevant stimuli. In a previous study, this oddball effect was much reduced when stimulus–response (S–R) mappings were still undefined at stimulus presentation (being later provided by response prompts). This reduction may reflect P3b’s dependence on transmitted information which might be any relevant information (informational value hypothesis) or, more specifically, information about how to respond (S–R link hypothesis). To distinguish between these two hypotheses and clarify their differences from classical stimulus evaluation hypothesis, we added a second dimension by presenting colored letters, with both colors and letters varying between a rare and a frequent alternative. Response prompts, presented half a second later, were, in different blocks, constant or variable across trials with respect to S–R mapping and with respect to the relevant dimension (color or letter). With partial information, when only one of these two factors is known at stimulus presentation (by being constant across trials), the hypotheses differ in their predictions. The oddball effect will be abolished according to S–R link hypothesis because knowledge of both factors is needed to determine the response, but will only be reduced according to informational value hypothesis and be fully maintained according to stimulus evaluation hypothesis. In fact, oddball effects only occurred with knowledge of both factors, i.e., if both the relevant dimension and its mapping to responses were constant across trials. These results confirm the preeminent role of knowledge about responses for eliciting P3.

Keywords

P3 S–R-mapping S–R link reactivation Informational value Stimulus evaluation 

Notes

Funding

This work was supported by funding granted to R.V. from Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (Ve110/17-1).

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no competing financial and other interests.

References

  1. Brunia CHM (2003) CNV and SPN: indices of anticipatory behavior. In: Jahanshahi M, Hallett M (eds) The Bereitschaftspotential: movement-related cortical potentials. Springer, Heidelberg, pp 207–227.  https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-0189-3_13 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Callaway E (1983) The pharmacology of human information processing. Psychophysiology 20:359–370.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1983.tb00915.x CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. De Jong R (1993) Multiple bottlenecks in overlapping task performance. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 19:965–980.  https://doi.org/10.1037/00961523.19.5.965 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Deiber MP, Ibañez V, Caldara R, Andrey C, Hauert CA (2005) Programming effectors and coordination in bimanual in-phase mirror finger movements. Cogn Brain Res 23:374–386.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.11.009 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Desmedt JE, Debecker J (1979) Wave form and neural mechanism of the decision P350 elicited without pre-stimulus CNV or readiness potential in random sequences of near-threshold auditory clicks and finger stimuli. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 47:648–670.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0013-4694(79)90293-1 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Dien J, Spencer KM, Donchin E (2004) Parsing the late positive complex: mental chronometry and the ERP components that inhabit the neighborhood of the P300. Psychophysiology 41:665–678.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2004.00193.x CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Donchin E, Coles MGH (1988) Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? Behav Brain Sci 11:357–374.  https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525X00058027 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Duncan-Johnson CC (1981) P300 latency: a new metric of information processing. Psychophysiology 18:207–215.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1981.tb03020.x CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Duncan-Johnson CC, Donchin E (1977) On quantifying surprise: the variation of event-related potentials with subjective probability. Psychophysiology 14:456–467.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1977.tb01312.x CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Frith CD, Done DJ (1986) Routes to action in reaction time tasks. Psychol Res 48:169–177.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00309165 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Gaeta H, Friedman D, Hunt G (2003) Stimulus characteristics and task category dissociate the anterior and posterior aspects of novelty P3. Psychophysiology 40:198–208.  https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-8986.00022 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Gratton G, Bosco CM, Kramer AF, Coles MGH, Wickens CD, Donchin E (1990) Event-related brain potentials as indices of information extraction and response priming. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 75:419–432.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0013-4694(90)90087-Z CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Hommel B, Müsseler J, Aschersleben G, Prinz W (2001) The theory of event coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. Behav Brain Sci 24:849–937.  https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X01000103 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Johnson R Jr (1986) A triarchic model of P300 amplitude. Psychophysiology 23:367–384.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1986.tb00649.x CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Kok A (1978) The effect of warning stimulus novelty on the P300 and components of the contingent negative variation. Biol Psychol 6:219–233.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0301-0511(78)90024-8 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Kutas M, McCarthy G, Donchin E (1977) Augmenting mental chronometry: the P300 as a measure of stimulus evaluation time. Science 197:792–795.  https://doi.org/10.1126/science.887923 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Lakens D (2013) Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: a practical primer for t-tests and ANOVAs. Front Psychol 4:863.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00863 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  18. Oberauer K (2010) Declarative and procedural working memory: Common principles, common capacity limits? Psychol Belg 50:277–308.  https://doi.org/10.5334/pb-50-3-4-277 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. O’Connell RG, Dockree PM, Kelly SP (2012) A supramodal accumulation-to-bound signal that determines perceptual decisions in humans. Nature Neurosci 15:1729–1735.  https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3248 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Ouyang G, Sommer W, Zhou C (2015) Updating and validating a new framework for restoring and analyzing latency-variable ERP components from single trials with residue iteration decomposition (RIDE). Psychophysiology 52:839–856.  https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12411 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Pashler H (1984) Processing stages in overlapping tasks: evidence for a central bottleneck. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 10:358–377.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.10.3.358 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Pashler H (1994) Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. Psychol Bull 116:220–244.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.116.2.220 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Polich J (2007) Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clin Neurophysiol 118:2128–2148.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2007.04.019 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  24. Rohrbaugh JW, Syndulko K, Lindsley DB (1976) Brain wave components of the contingent negative variation in humans. Science 191:1055–1057.  https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1251217 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Ruthruff E, Pashler HE, Klaassen A (2001) Processing bottlenecks in dual-task performance: structural limitation or strategic postponement? Psychonom Bull Rev 8:73–80.  https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196141 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Sawaki R, Katayama J (2009) Difficulty of discrimination modulates attentional capture by regulating attentional focus. J Cogn Neurosci 21:359–371.  https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.21022 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Scharf F, Nestler S (2018) Principles behind variance misallocation in temporal exploratory factor analysis for ERP data: insights from an inter-factor covariance decomposition. Int J Psychophysiol 128:119–136.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2018.03.019 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Squires NK, Squires KC, Hillyard SA (1975) Two varieties of long-latency positive waves evoked by unpredictable auditory stimuli in man. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 38:387–401.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0013-4694(75)90263-1 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Sutton S (1979) P300—thirteen years later. In: Begleiter H (ed) Evoked brain potentials and behavior. Plenum Press, New York, pp 107–126.  https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4684-3462-0_4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Sutton S, Tueting P, Zubin J, John ER (1967) Information delivery and the sensory evoked potential. Science 155:1436–1439.  https://doi.org/10.1126/science.155.3768.1436 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Twomey DM, Kelly SP, O’Connell RGO (2016) Abstract and effector-selective decision signals exhibit qualitatively distinct dynamics before delayed perceptual reports. J Neurosci 36:7346–7352.  https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.4162-15.2016 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  32. Verleger R (1997) On the utility of P3 latency as an index of mental chronometry. Psychophysiology 34:131–156.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1997.tb02125.x CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Verleger R, Śmigasiewicz K (2016) Do rare stimuli evoke large P3s by being unexpected? A comparison of the oddball effects between standard-oddball and prediction-oddball tasks. Adv Cogn Psychol 12:88–104.  https://doi.org/10.5709/acp-0189-9 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  34. Verleger R, Wauschkuhn B, van der Lubbe RHJ, Jaśkowski P, Trillenberg P (2000) Posterior and anterior contributions of hand-movement preparation to Late CNV. J Psychophysiol 14:69–86.  https://doi.org/10.1027//0269-8803.14.2.69 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Verleger R, Metzner MF, Ouyang G, Śmigasiewicz K, Zhou C (2014) Testing the stimulus-to-response bridging function of the oddball-P3 by delayed response signals and residue iteration decomposition (RIDE). NeuroImage 100:271–280.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.06.036 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Verleger R, Hamann LM, Asanowicz D, Śmigasiewicz K (2015) Testing the S–R link hypothesis of P3b: the oddball effect on S1-evoked P3 gets reduced by increased task relevance of S2. Biol Psychol 108:25–35.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.02.010 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Verleger R, Grauhan N, Śmigasiewicz K (2016a) Is P3 a strategic or a tactical component? Relationships of P3 sub-components to response times in oddball tasks with go, no-go and choice responses. NeuroImage 143:223–234.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.08.049 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Verleger R, Grauhan N, Śmigasiewicz K (2016b) Effects of response delays and of unknown stimulus–response mappings on the oddball effect on P3. Psychophysiology 53:1858–1869.  https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12756 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Verleger R, Siller B, Ouyang G, Śmigasiewicz K (2017) Effects on P3 of spreading targets and response prompts apart. Biol Psychol 126:1–11.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.03.011 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Walter WG, Cooper R, Aldridge VJ, McCallum WC, Winter AL (1964) Contingent negative variation: an electric sign of sensorimotor association and expectancy in the human brain. Nature 203:380–384.  https://doi.org/10.1038/203380a0 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Wild-Wall N, Sangals J, Sommer W, Leuthold H (2003) Are fingers special? Evidence about movement preparation from event-related potentials. Psychophysiology 40:7–16.  https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-8986.00002 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of NeurologyUniversity of LübeckLübeckGermany
  2. 2.Institute of Psychology IIUniversity of LübeckLübeckGermany
  3. 3.Department of PsychologyGoethe University of FrankfurtFrankfurtGermany
  4. 4.Laboratoire de Neuroscience CognitiveAix-Marseille Université, CNRSMarseilleFrance

Personalised recommendations