Advertisement

Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 236, Issue 10, pp 2589–2601 | Cite as

Training children aged 5–10 years in compliance control: tracing smaller figures yields better learning not specific to the scale of drawn figures

  • Winona Snapp-Childs
  • Aaron J. Fath
  • Geoffrey P. Bingham
Research Article
  • 33 Downloads

Abstract

Previously we developed a method that supports active movement generation to allow practice with improvement of good compliance control in tracing and drawing. We showed that the method allowed children with motor impairments to improve at a 3D tracing task to become as proficient as typically developing children and that the training improved 2D figure copying. In this study, we expanded the training protocol to include a wider variety of ages (5–10-year-olds) and we made the figures traced in training the same as in figure copying, but varied the scale of training and copying figures to assess the generality of learning. Forty-eight children were assigned to groups trained using large or small figures. All were tested before training with a tracing task and a copying task. Then, the children trained over five sessions in the tracing task with either small or large figures. Finally, the tracing and copying tasks were tested again following training. A mean speed measure was used to control for path length variations in the timed task. Performance on both tasks at both baseline and posttest varied as a function of the size of the figure and age. In addition, tracing performance also varied with the level of support. In particular, speeds were higher with more support, larger figures and older children. After training, performance improved. Speeds increased. In tracing, performance improved more for large figures traced by children who trained on large figures. In copying, however, performance only improved significantly for children who had trained on small figures and it improved equally for large and small figures. In conclusion, training by tracing smaller figures yielded better learning that was not, however, specific to the scale of drawn figures. Small figures exhibit greater mean curvature. We infer that it yielded better general improvement.

Keywords

Manual control Compliance control Prospective control Motor development Specificity 

Notes

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by NICHD R01HD070832.

References

  1. Adolph KE, Robinson SR (2015) Motor development. In: Liben L, Muller U (eds) Handbook of child psychology and developmental science: vol 2: cognitive processes, 7th edn. Wiley, New York, pp 147–170Google Scholar
  2. Asher AV (2006) Handwriting instruction in elementary schools. Am J Occup Ther 60:461–471CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Bernstein NA (1967) The co-ordination and regulation of movements. Pergamon Press Ltd, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  4. Common Core State Standards Initiative (2009). http://www.corestandards.org/. Accessed 1 Mar 2015
  5. Culmer PR, Levesley MC, Mon-Williams M, Williams JHG (2009) A new tool for assessing human movement: the kinematic assessment tool. J Neurosci Methods 184(1):184–192.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2009.07.025 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Graham S (1990) The role of production factors in learning disabled students’ compositions. J Educ Psychol 82:781–791CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Graham S, Harris KR (2009) Almost 30 years of writing research: making sense of it all with The Wrath of Khan. Learn Disabil Res Pract 24(2):58–68.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5826.2009.01277.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Graham S, Berninger V, Weintraub N, Schafer W (1998) The development of handwriting speed and legibility in grades 1–9. J Educ Res 92(1):42–52CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Graham S, Harris KR, Mason L, Fink-chorzempa B, Moran S, Saddler B (2008a) How do primary grade teachers teach handwriting ? A national survey. Read Writ 21:49–69.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-007-9064-z CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Graham S, Morphy P, Harris KR, Fink-Chorzempa B, Saddler B, Moran S, Mason L (2008b) Teaching spelling in the primary grades: a national survey of instructional practices and adaptations. Am Educ Res J 45(3):796–825.  https://doi.org/10.3102/0002831208319722 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Henderson SE, Sugden DA, Barnett AL (2007) Movement assessment battery for children-2: movement ABC-2: examiner’s manual, 2nd edn. Pearson Assessment, LondonGoogle Scholar
  12. Hogan N (1985) The mechanics of multi-joint posture and movement control. Biol Cybern 52:315–331CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Hogan N (1990) Mechanical impedance of single- and multi-articular systems. In: Multiple muscle systems. Springer, New York, pp 149–164CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. James KH, James TW, Jobard G, Wong ACN, Gauthier I (2005) Letter processing in the visual system: different activation patterns for single letters and strings. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 5(4):452–466CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Kersey AJ, James KH (2013) Brain activation patterns resulting from learning letter forms through active self-production and passive observation in young children. Front Psychol 4:567.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00567 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  16. Lacquaniti F, Terzuolo C, Viviani PP (1983) The law relating the kinematic and figural aspects of drawing movements. Acta Physiol (Oxf) 54(1–3):115–130.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(83)90027-6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Mandelstam J (2015) Should kids learn handwriting? An IU scientist thinks so. Bloom Magazine. http://www.magbloom.com/2015/02/should-kids-learn-handwriting-an-iu-scientist-thinks-so/
  18. Merton PA (1972) How we control the contraction of our muscles. Sci Am 226(5):30–37CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Myronenko A, Song X (2010) Point set registration: coherent point drift. IEEE Trans Pattern Anal Mach Intell 32(12):2262–2275.  https://doi.org/10.1109/TPAMI.2010.46 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Newell KM, Shapiro DC, Carlton MJ (1979) Coordinating visual and kinaesthetic memory codes. Br J Psychol 70(1):87–96.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1979.tb02147.x CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Phillips JG, Ogeil RP, Best C (2009) Motor constancy and the upsizing of handwriting. Hum Mov Sci 28(5):578–587.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2009.07.004 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Proteau L, Marteniuk RG, Lévesque L (1992) A sensorimotor basis for motor learning: evidence indicating specificity of practice. Q J Exp Psychol Sect A 44(3):557–575.  https://doi.org/10.1080/14640749208401298 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Snapp-Childs W, Casserly E, Mon-Williams M, Bingham GP (2013a) Active prospective control is required for effective sensorimotor learning. PLoS One 8(10):e77609.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0077609 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  24. Snapp-Childs W, Mon-Williams M, Bingham GP (2013b) A sensorimotor approach to the training of manual actions in children with developmental coordination disorder. J Child Neurol 28(2):204–212.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0883073812461945 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Snapp-Childs W, Flatters I, Fath A, Mon-Williams M, Bingham GP (2014) Training compliance control yields improvements in drawing as a function of beery scores. PLoS One 9(3):e92464.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0092464 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  26. Snapp-Childs W, Wang XM, Bingham GP (2016a) Progressive reduction versus fixed level of support during training: when less is less. Hum Mov Sci 45:172–181.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2015.11.010 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Snapp-Childs W, Wang XM, Bingham GP (2016b) Progressive reduction versus fixed level of support during training: when less is less. Hum Mov Sci.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2015.11.010 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  28. Vinci-Booher SA, James KH (2016) Neural substrates of sensorimotor processes: letter writing and letter perception. J Neurophysiol 115(1):1–4.  https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.01042.2014 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. von Hofsten C (1993) Prospective control: a basic aspect of action development. Hum Dev 36(5):253–270CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Weintraub N, Graham S (1998) Writing legibly and quickly: a study of children’s ability to adjust their handwriting to meet common classroom demands. Learn Disabil Res Pract 13:146–152Google Scholar
  31. Wilson BN, Crawford SG, Green D, Roberts G, Aylott A, Kaplan BJ (2009) Psychometric properties of the revised developmental coordination disorder questionnaire. Phys Occup Ther Pediatr 29(2):182–202.  https://doi.org/10.1080/01942630902784761 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Zhao H, Warren WH (2015) On-line and model-based approaches to the visual control of action. Vis Res 110:190–202CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Psychological and Brain SciencesIndiana UniversityBloomingtonUSA

Personalised recommendations