Advertisement

Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 236, Issue 10, pp 2603–2610 | Cite as

Attention orienting near the hand following performed and imagined actions

  • John P. Garza
  • Catherine L. Reed
  • Ralph J. RobertsJr.
Research Article
  • 90 Downloads

Abstract

Recent studies have documented that the hand’s ability to perform actions affects the visual processing and attention for objects near the hand, suggesting that actions may have specific effects on visual orienting. However, most research on the relation between spatial attention and action focuses on actions as responses to visual attention manipulations. The current study examines visual attention immediately following an executed or imagined action. A modified spatial cuing paradigm tested whether a brief, lateralized hand-pinch performed by a visually hidden hand near the target location, facilitated or inhibited subsequent visual target detection. Conditions in which hand-pinches were fully executed (action) were compared to ones with no hand-pinch (inaction) in Experiment 1 and imagined pinches (imagine) in Experiment 2. Results from Experiment 1 indicated that performed hand pinches facilitated rather than inhibited subsequent detection responses to targets appearing near the pinch, but target detection was not affected by inaction. In Experiment 2, both action and imagined action conditions cued attention and facilitated responses, but along differing time courses. These results highlight the ongoing nature of visual attention and demonstrate how it is deployed to locations even following actions.

Keywords

Hand proximity effect Visual cuing Attention Action execution Imagined actions Motor programming 

References

  1. Abrams RA, Davoli CC, Du F, Knapp WH, Paull D (2008) Altered vision near the hands. Cognition 107:1035–1047CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Berthoz A (1991) Brain mechanisms in the perception and control of movement. In: Orban GA, Wolf S, Hillsdale NJ (eds) Cognitive neuroscience: research directions in cognitive neuroscience: European perspectives, vol 4. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc, England, pp 43–71Google Scholar
  3. Christensen A, Ilg W, Giese MA (2011) Spatiotemporal tuning of the facilitation of biological motion perception by concurrent motor execution. J Neurosci 31:3493–3499CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Cosman JD, Vecera SP (2010) Attention affects visual perceptual processing near the hand. Psychol Sci 21:1254–1258CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Cousineau D (2005) Confidence intervals in within-subjects designs: a simpler solution to Loftus and Masson’s method. Tutor Quant Methods Psychol 1:42–45CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Decety J (1996) Do imagined and executed actions share the same neural substrate? Cogn Brain Res 3:87–93CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Desimone R, Duncan J (1995) Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annu Rev Neurosci 18:193–222CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Deubel H, Schneider WX, Paprotta (1998) Selective dorsal and ventral processing: evidence for a common attentional mechanism in reaching and perception. Vis Cogn 5:81–107CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Duncan J (2006) Brain mechanisms of attention. Q J Exp Psychol 59:2–27CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang A-G, Buchner A (2007) G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods 39:175–191CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Frak V, Paulignan Y, Jeannerod M (2001) Orientation of the opposition axis in mentally simulated grasping. Exp Brain Res 136:120–127CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Garza J, Strom M, Wright CE, Roberts R, Reed CL (2013) Top-down influences mediate hand bias in spatial attention. Atten Percept Psychophys 75:819–823CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Gherri E, Eimer M (2010) Manual response preparation disrupts spatial attention: an electrophysiological investigation of links between action and attention. Neuropsychologia 48:961–969CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Grubb JD, Reed CL (2002) Trunk Orientation induces neglect-like lateral biases in covert attention. Psychol Sci 13:553–556CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Grubb J, Reed CL, Bate S, Garza JP, Roberts RJ Jr (2008) Walking reveals trunk orientation bias for visual attention. Percept Psychophys 70:688–696CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Herwig A (2015) Linking perception and action by structure or process? Toward an integrative perspective. Neurosci Behav Rev 52:105–116CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Hommel B (2004) Event files: feature binding in and across perception and across. Trends Cogn Sci 8:494–500CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Hommel B, Müsseler J, Aschersleben G, Prinz W (2001) The theory of event coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. Behav Brain Sci 24:849–878CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Howard LA, Tipper SP (1997) Hand deviations away from visual cues: indirect evidence for inhibition. Exp Brain Res 113:144–152CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Jeannerod M (1995) Mental imagery in the motor cortex. Neuropsychologia 33:1419–1432CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Kennett S, Spence C, Driver J (2002) Visuo-tactile links in covert exogenous spatial attention remap across changes in unseen hand posture. Percept Psychophys 64:1083–1094CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Klein RM (2000) Inhibition of return. Trends Cogn Sci 4(4):138–147CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Luck SJ, Vecera SP (2002) Attention. In: Pashler H, Yantis S (eds) Steven’s handbook of experimental psychology, vol 1 sensation and perception, 3rd edn. Wiley, New York, pp 235–286Google Scholar
  24. MacKay DG (1987) The organization of perception and action: a theory for language and other cognitive skills. Springer, Berlin, p 255CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Posner MI, Cohen Y (1984). Components of visual orienting. In Bouma H, Bowhuis D (eds) Attention and performance X. Hillsdale, Erlbaum, pp 531–556Google Scholar
  26. Posner MI, Rafal RD, Choate LS, Vaughan J (1985) Inhibition of return: neural basis and function. Cogn Neuropsychol 2:211–228CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Pratt J, Abrams RA (1994) Action-centered inhibition: effects of distractors on movement planning and execution. Hum Mov Sci 13:245–254CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Prinz W (1997) Perception and action planning. Eur J Cogn Psychol 9:129–154CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Reed CL (2002) Chronometric comparisons of imagery to action: visualizing vs. physically performing springboard dives. Mem Cogn 30:1169–1178CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Reed CL, Grubb JD, Steele C (2006) Hands up: attentional prioritization of space near the hand. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 32:166–177CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Reed CL, Betz R, Garza J, Roberts R (2010) Grab it! Biased attention in functional hand and tool space. Atten Percept Psychophys 72:236–245CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Ristic J, Wright A, Kingstone A (2007) Attentional control and reflexive orienting to gaze and arrow cues. Psychon Bull Rev 14:964–969CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Rizzolatti G, Riggio L, Dascola I, Umiltá C (1987) Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention. Neuropsychologia 25:31–40CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Schiegg A, Deubel H, Schneider WX (2003) Attentional selection during preparation of prehension movements. Vis Cogn 10:409–431CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Spence C, Pavani F, Driver J (2000) Crossmodal links between vision and touch in covert endogenous spatial attention. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 26(4):1298–1319CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Taylor TL, Klein RM (1998) On the causes and effects of inhibition of return. Psychon Bull Rev 5(4):625–643CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Thomas LE (2015) Grasp posture alters visual processing biases near the hands. Psychol Sci 26:625–632CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  38. Tipper SP (1992) Selection for action: the role of inhibitory mechanisms. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 1:105–109CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Tipper SP, Howard LA, Houghton G (1999) Action-based mechanisms of attention. In: Humphreys GW, Duncan J, Treisman A (eds) Attention, space, and action: studies in cognitive neuroscience. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 232–247Google Scholar
  40. Treisman A (2004) Psychological issues in selective attention. In: The cognitive neurosciences, 3rd edn. MIT, Cambridge, pp 529–544Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  • John P. Garza
    • 1
  • Catherine L. Reed
    • 2
  • Ralph J. RobertsJr.
    • 3
  1. 1.University of Texas at El PasoEl PasoUSA
  2. 2.Department of Psychology, Claremont McKenna CollegeClaremont Graduate UniversityClaremontUSA
  3. 3.Department of PsychologyUniversity of DenverDenverUSA

Personalised recommendations