Advertisement

Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 236, Issue 9, pp 2507–2518 | Cite as

Boredom, sustained attention and the default mode network

  • James Danckert
  • Colleen Merrifield
Research Article

Abstract

Boredom is a ubiquitous human experience that can best be described as an inability to engage with one’s environment despite the motivation to do so. Boredom is perceived as a negative experience and demonstrates strong associations with other negatively valenced states including depression and aggression. Although boredom has been shown to be elevated in neurological and psychiatric illnesses, little is known about the neural underpinnings of the state. We scanned the brains of healthy participants under four separate conditions: a resting state scan, a sustained attention task and two video-based mood inductions, one known to produce boredom and another we validated to produce a state of interest or engagement. Using independent components analyses, results showed common regions of correlated activation in posterior regions of the so-called default mode network (DMN) of the brain across all four conditions. The sustained attention and boredom induction scans were differentiated from the resting state scan by the presence of anticorrelated activity—i.e. when DMN regions were active, this region was deactivated—in the anterior insula cortex. This same region demonstrated correlated activity with both the DMN and the regions associated with attentional control during the interest mood induction. We interpret these findings to suggest that boredom represents a failure to engage executive control networks when faced with a monotonous task—in other words, when the task demands some level of engagement (watch the movie, search for infrequent targets), but is so mundane that attempts to do so fail.

Keywords

Boredom Sustained attention Default mode network Anterior insula 

References

  1. Andrews-Hanna JR (2012) The brain’s default network and its adaptive role in internal mentation. Neuroscientist 18:251–270CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Barmack JE (1939) A definition of boredom: a reply to Mr. Berman. Am J Psychol 52:467–471Google Scholar
  3. Baumeister RF, Vohs KD (2003) Self-regulation and the executive function of the self. In: Leary MR, Tangney JP (eds) Handbook of self and identity. Guilford Press, New York, pp 197–217Google Scholar
  4. Baumeister RF, Heatherton TF, Tice DM (1994) Losing control: how and why people fail at self-regulation. Academic Press, San DiegoGoogle Scholar
  5. Binder JR (2012) Task-induced deactivation of the “resting” state. Neuroimage 62:1086–1091CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Binder JR, Frost JA, Hammeke TA, Bellgowan PS, Rao SM, Cox RW (1999) Conceptual processing during the conscious resting state: a functional MRI study. J Cogn Neurosci 11:80–95CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Bonnelle V, Leech R, Kinnunen KM, Ham TE, Beckmann CF et al (2011) Default mode network connectivity predicts sustained attention deficits after traumatic brain injury. J Neurosci 31:13422–13451CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Buckner RL, Andrews-Hanna JR, Schacter DL (2008) The brain’s default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann NY Acad Sci 1124:1–39CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Cabeza R, Anderson ND, Locantore JK, McIntosh AR (2002) Aging gracefully: compensatory brain activity in high-performing adults. NeuorImage 17:1394–1402CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Carriere JSA, Cheyne JA, Smilek D (2008) Everyday attention lapses and memory failures: the affective consequences of mindlessness. Conscious Cogn 17:835–847CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Cheyne JA, Carriere JSA, Smilek D (2006) Absent-mindedness: lapses of conscious awareness and everyday cognitive failures. Conscious Cogn 15:578–592CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Christoff K (2011) Undirected thought: neural determinants and correlates. Brain Res 1428:51–59CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Christoff K, Gordon AM, Smallwood J, Smith R, Schooler JW (2009) Experience sampling during fMRI reveals default network and executive system contributions to mind-wandering. Proc Natl Acad Sci 106:8719–8724CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Csikszentmihalyi M (1975) Beyond boredom and anxiety. Jossey-Bass, San FranciscoGoogle Scholar
  15. Culham JC, Danckert SL, DeSouza JFX, Gati JS, Menon RS, Goodale MA (2003) Visually guided grasping produces fMRI activation in dorsal but not ventral stream brain areas. Exp Brain Res 153:180–189CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Damrad-Frye R, Laird JD (1989) The experience of boredom: the role of the self-perception of attention. J Pers Soc Psychol 57:315–320CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Danckert JA, Allman AA (2005) Time flies when you’re having fun: temporal estimation and the experience of boredom. Brain Cogn 59:236–245CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Daselaar SM, Fleck MS, Dobbins IG, Madden DJ, Cabeza R (2006) Effects of healthy aging on hippocampal and rhinal memory functions: an event-related fMRI study. Cereb Cortex 16:1771–1782CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  19. De Martino F, Gentile F, Esposito F, Balsi M, Di Salle F et al (2007) Classification of fMRI independent components using IC-fingerprints and support vector machine classifiers. NeuroImage 34:177–194CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Eastwood JD, Frischen A, Fenske MJ, Smilek D (2012) The unengaged mind: defining boredom in terms of attention. Perspect Psychol Sci 7:482–495CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Esposito F, Scarabino T, Hyvarinen A, Himberg J, Formisano E et al (2005) Independent components analysis of fMRI group studies by self-organizing clustering. NeuroImage 25:193–205CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Farmer R, Sundberg ND (1986) Boredom proneness: the development and correlates of a new scale. J Pers Assess 50:4–17CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Fothergill A, Berlowitz V, Malone S, Lemire M (2007) Planet Earth. Warner Home Video, Burbank, CA, USAGoogle Scholar
  24. Fox KC, Thompson E, Andrews-Hanna JR, Christoff K (2014) Is thinking really aversive? Commentary on Wilson et al.’s “Just think: the challenges of the disengaged mind”. Front Psychol 5:1427. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01427 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  25. Fox KC, Spreng RN, Ellamil M, Andrews-Hanna JR, Christoff K (2015) The wandering brain: meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of mind-wandering and related spontaneous thought processes. Neuroimage 111:611–621CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Goldberg YK, Eastwood JD, La Guardia JG, Danckert J (2011) Boredom: an emotional experience distinct from apathy, anhedonia, or depression. J Soc Clin Psychol 30:647–666CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Greicius MD, Krasnow B, Reiss AL, Menon V (2003) Functional connectivity in the resting brain: a network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proc Natl Acad Sci 100:253–258CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Gusnard DA, Raichle ME (2001) Searching for a baseline: functional imaging and the resting human brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 2:685–694CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Gusnard DA, Akbudak E, Shulman GL, Raichle ME (2001) Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: relation to a default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci 98:4259–4264CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Hyvarinen A, Hoyer PO, Inki M (2001) Topographic independent components analysis. Neural Comput 13:1527–1558CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Isacescu J, Struk A, Danckert J (2016) Cognitive and affective predictors of boredom proneness. Emotion (revise and resubmit)Google Scholar
  32. Killingsworth MA, Gilbert DT (2010) A wandering mind is an unhappy mind. Science 330:932CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Lawrence NS, Ross TJ, Hoffmann R, Garavan H, Stein EA (2003) Multiple neuronal networks mediate sustained attention. J Cogn Neurosci 15:1028–1038CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. London H, Monell L (1974) Cognitive manipulations of boredom. In: London H, Nisbett R (eds) Thought and feeling. Aldine, Chicago, pp 44–59Google Scholar
  35. Malkovsky E, Merrifield C, Goldberg Y, Danckert J (2012) Exploring the relationship between boredom and sustained attention. Exp Brain Res 221:59–67CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Mason MF, Norton MI, Van Horn JD, Wegner DM, Grafton ST, Macrae CN (2007) Wandering minds: the default network and stimulus-independent thought. Science 315:393–395CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  37. Menon V, Uddin LQ (2010) Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function. Brain Struct Funct 214:655–667CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  38. Merrifield C, Danckert J (2014) Characterising the psychophysiological signature of boredom. Exp Brain Res 232:481–491CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Morcom AM, Fletcher PC (2007a) Does the brain have a baseline? Why we should be resisting a rest. NeuroImage 37:1073–1082CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Morcom AM, Fletcher PC (2007b) Cognitive neuroscience: the case for design rather than default. NeuroImage 37:1097–1099CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Nakamura J, Csikszentmihalyi M (2002) The concept of flow. In: Snyder CR, Lopez SJ (eds) Handbook of positive psychology. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 89–105Google Scholar
  42. Ozen LJ, Itier RJ, Preston FF, Fernandes MA (2013) Long-term working memory deficits after concussion: electrophysiological evidence. Brain Inj 27:1244–1255CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Pattyn N, Neyt X, Henderickx D, Soetens E (2008) Psychophysiological investigation of the vigilance decrement: boredom or cognitive fatigue? Phys Behav 93:369–378CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard DA, Shulman GL (2001) A default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci 98:676–682CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Rawn CD, Vohs KD (2006) The importance of self-regulation for interpersonal functioning. In: Vohs KD, Finkel EJ (eds) Self and relationships: connecting intrapersonal and interpersonal processes. Guilford Press, New York, pp 15–31Google Scholar
  46. Rizzo M, Robin DA (1990) Simultanagnosia: a defect of sustained attention yields insights on visual information-processing. Neurology 40:447–455CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Sansone C, Weir C, Harpster L, Morgan C (1992) Once a boring task always a boring task? Interest as a self-regulatory mechanism. J Pers Soc Psych 63:370–390CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Scerbo MW (1998) What’s so boring about vigilance? In: Hoffman RR, Sherrick MF, Warm JS (eds) Viewing psychology as a whole: the integrative science of William N. Dember. American Psychological Association, Washington, DC, pp 135–166Google Scholar
  49. Seli P, Carriere JS, Smilek D (2015) Not all mind-wandering is created equal: dissociating deliberate from spontaneous mind-wandering. Psychol Res 79:750–758CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Smallwood J, O’Connor RC, Sudbery MV, Obonsawin M (2007) Mind-wandering and dysphoria. Cogn Emot 21:816–842CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Smith JL, Wagaman J, Handley IM (2009) Keeping it dull or making it fun: task variation as a function of promotion versus prevention focus. Motiv Emot 33:150–160CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Stawarczyk D, Majerus S, Maquet P, D’Argembeau A (2011) Neural correlates of ongoing conscious experience: both task-unrelatedness and stimulus-independence are related to default network activity. PLoS ONE 6:16997. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0016997 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Struk AA, Scholer AA, Danckert J (2015a) A Self-regulatory approach to understanding boredom proneness. Cogn Emot. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2015.1064363
  54. Struk AA, Carriere J, Cheyne JA, Danckert J (2015b) A short boredom proneness scale (sBPS): psychometric properties. Assessment. doi: 10.1177/1073191115609996 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Talairach J, Tournoux P (1988) Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain. Thieme, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  56. Thackray RI, Bailey JP, Touchstone RM (1977) Physiological, subjective, and performance correlates of reported boredom and monotony while performing a simulated radar control task. In: Mackie RR (ed) Vigilance: theory, operational performance and physiological correlates. Plenum, New York, pp 203–216CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Ulrich M, Keller J, Grön, G (2015) Neural signatures of experimentally induced flow experiences identified in a typical fMRI block design with BOLD imaging. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 1–12Google Scholar
  58. Ulrich M, Keller J, Hoenig K, Waller C, Grön G (2014) Neural correlates of experimentally induced flow experiences. NeuroImage 86:194–202CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  59. van Tilburg W, Igou E (2011) On boredom and social identity: a pragmatic meaning-regulation approach. Pers Soc Psych Bull 37:1679–1691CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Weissman DH, Roberts KC, Visscher KM, Woldorff MG (2006) The neural bases of momentary lapses in attention. Nat Neurosci 9:971–978CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  61. Wilson TD, Reinhard DA, Westgate EC, Gilbert DT, Ellerbeck N, Hahn C, Brown CL, Shaked A (2014) Social psychology. Just think: the challenges of the disengaged mind. Science 345:75–77CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of WaterlooWaterlooCanada

Personalised recommendations