Skip to main content

Reliability and Deception in Language

  • Living reference work entry
  • First Online:

Synonyms

Costly signalling; Deceit; Honest signalling; Honesty; Reputation

Definition

In general terms, honest signalling theory seeks to explain what keeps animal communication an honest and reliable indicator of health, condition, and genetic quality in the face of many incentives for deception.

Introduction

Many commentators on animal communication and human language evolution have suggested that several aspects of language can be characterized in terms of honest signalling theory. Granted, although the complexity of language and human social interactions make plausible the suggestion that human utterances may have many parameters that could conceivably be assessed by receivers, I nonetheless selectively focus here on the most evidence-based proposals current to the honest signalling literature. Namely, the particular honest signals to be reviewed here include three related areas of research: (1) voice pitch, (2) vocabulary size, and (3) unfavorable social reputation.

Human Language...

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

References

  • Abeler, J., Becker, A., & Falk, A. (2012). Truth-telling: A representative assessment (pp. 1–18). Institute for the Study of Labor (IZA), no. 6919.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andersson, M. (1994). Sexual selection. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andersson, M. (1986). Evolution of condition-dependent sex ornaments and mating preferences: Sexual selection based on viability differences. Evolution, 40, 804–816.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bock, W. J. (2000). Explanatory history of the origin of feathers. American Zoologist, 40(4), 478–485.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1992). Cognitive adaptations for social exchange. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind (pp. 163–228). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Waal, F. (2005). Intentional deception in primates. Evolutionary Anthropology, 1(3), 86–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R. I. M. (2009). Why only humans have language. In R. Botha & C. Knight (Eds.), The prehistory of language (pp. 12–36). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Fitch, W. T. (2010). The evolution of language. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hauser, M. D. (1992). Costs of deception: Cheaters are punished in rhesus monkeys. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 89(24), 12137–12139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lange, B. P., Zaretsky, E., Schwarz, S., & Euler, H. A. (2014). Words won’t fail: Experimental evidence on the role of verbal proficiency in mate choice. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 33(5), 482–499.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • le Roux, A., Snyder-Mackler, N., Roberts, E. K., Beehner, J. C., & Bergman, T. J. (2013). Evidence for tactical concealment in a wild primate. Nature Communications, 4(1462), 1–6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. (2000). The mating mind: How sexual choice shaped the evolution of human nature. London: Heineman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Millikan, R. (1984). Language, thought and other biological categories. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Connor, J. J. M., Re, D. E., & Feinberg, D. R. (2011). Voice pitch influences perceptions of sexual infidelity. Evolutionary Psychology, 9, 64–78.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oesch, N. (2016). Deception as a derived function of language. Frontiers in Psychology, 7(1485), 1–7.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oesch, N. (in revision). Honest signalling theory and human language: Receiver-dependent and receiver-independent costs. Journal of Evolutionary Biology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oesch, N., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (in revision). The adaptive significance of human language. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pisanski, K., Cartei, V., McGettigan, C., Raine, J., & Reby, D. (2016). Voice modulation: A window into the origins of human vocal control? Trends in Cognitive Science, 20(4), 304–318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plomin, R., & Deary, I. J. (2015). Genetics and intelligence differences: Five special findings. Molecular Psychiatry, 20, 98–108.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., Knopik, V. S. & Neiderhiser, J. M. (2013). Behavioral Genetics. 6th ed. New York: Worth Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Puts, D. A., Hill, A. K., Bailey, D. H., Walker, R. S., Rendall, D., Wheatley, J. R., Welling, L. L. M., Dawood, K., Cárdenas, R., Burriss, R. P., Jablonski, N. G., Shriver, M. D., Weiss, D., Lameira, A. R., Apicella, C. L., Owren, M. J., Barelli, C., Glenn, M. E., & Ramos-Fernandez, G. (2016). Sexual selection on male vocal fundamental frequency in humans and other anthropoids. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 283(1829), 1–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenberg, J., & Tunney, R. J. (2008). Human vocabulary use as display. Evolutionary Psychology, 6(3), 538–549.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Searcy, W. A., & Nowicki, S. (2005). The evolution of animal communication: Reliability and deception in signalling systems. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Serota, K. B., Levine, T. R., & Boster, F. J. (2010). The prevalence of lying in America: Studies of self-reported lies. Human Communication Research, 36(1), 2–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Silk, J. B., Kaldor, E., & Boyd, R. (2000). Cheap talk when interests conflict. Animal Behaviour, 59(2), 423–432.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spinath, F. M., & Gottschling, J. (2015). Genetics of cognitive abilities. In J. D. Wright (Ed.), International encyclopedia of the social and behavioural sciences (Vol. 12, 2nd ed., pp. 297–302). Oxford: Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Tibbetts, E. A., & Dale, J. (2004). A socially enforced signal of quality in a paper wasp. Nature, 432, 218–222.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Titze, I. R. (2000). Principles of voice production. Iowa City: National Center for Voice and Speech.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toma, C., Hancock, J. T., & Ellison, N. B. (2008). Separating fact from fiction: An examination of deceptive self-presentation in online dating profiles. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 1023–1036.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Trzaskowski, M., Davis, O. S., DeFries, J. C., Yang, J., Visscher, P. M., & Plomin, R. (2013). DNA evidence for strong genome-wide pleiotropy of cognitive and learning abilities. Behavior Genetics, 43, 267–273.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • van den Berg, S. M., Posthuma, D., & Boomsma, D. I. (2004). A longitudinal genetic study of vocabulary knowledge in adults. Twin Research, 3, 284–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Nathan Oesch .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Section Editor information

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2017 Springer International Publishing AG

About this entry

Cite this entry

Oesch, N. (2017). Reliability and Deception in Language. In: Shackelford, T., Weekes-Shackelford, V. (eds) Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_3301-1

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_3301-1

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-16999-6

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-16999-6

  • eBook Packages: Springer Reference Behavioral Science and PsychologyReference Module Humanities and Social SciencesReference Module Business, Economics and Social Sciences

Publish with us

Policies and ethics