Introduction

Neuropsychiatric conditions such as intellectual disability (ID), autism spectrum disorder (ASD), and schizophrenia are among the most common and debilitating of human disorders, yet they are also among the least well understood. All three conditions are known to have a strong genetic component [1]; and with recent advances in technology, the genetic risk factors underlying this heritability are gradually being identified. Despite the diagnostic differences, many of the genetic risk factors are shared among these disorders.

In this review, we will consider the phenotypic overlap associated with four of the best-characterized neuropsychiatric genetic risk factors, specifically copy number variants (CNVs) at 22q11.2, 15q11.2-13.1 (BP2-3), 16p11.2 (BP4-5), and 7q11.23 (Table 1). We will consider how understanding this overlap can inform our understanding of the architecture of risk in these conditions, and the implications as specific genes are implicated with each disorder.

Table 1 Characteristics of eight well-characterized CNVs associated with neuropsychiatric disorders at four loci

Chromosomal Abnormalities and Copy Number Variants (CNVs)

Historically syndromes caused by chromosomal abnormalities, identified through karyotype analysis, have been associated with several neuropsychiatric conditions. For example, Down syndrome, caused by an extra copy of chromosome 21 [2] is a common cause of ID and, less commonly, of ASD [3].

Alongside detecting abnormal numbers of chromosomes, karyotype analysis is also used to identify chromosomal rearrangements, such as translocations, where large segments of DNA are swapped between chromosomes or within the same chromosome. Such translocations were used to demonstrate that a region on the long arm of chromosome 22 [4, 5] was involved in DiGeorge syndrome, a condition that is frequently associated with ID [6] alongside numerous other syndromic features.

As technology has progressed, it has become possible to identify smaller changes in chromosomal structure such as deletions and duplications, collectively called copy number variations (CNVs). High-resolution karyotyping allows genome-wide detection of large CNVs (e.g., >2–5 Mb), while fluorescence in situ hybridization (FISH) allows targeted detection of sub-microscopic CNVs (e.g., >100 kb). Many such CNVs are clinically relevant, for example, deletion CNVs on the long arm of chromosome 22 (22q11.2) are a more common cause of DiGeorge syndrome than the translocations identified by karyotype analysis [7].

The advent of chromosomal microarrays has allowed reliable detection of smaller CNVs (e.g., >30 kb) in a genome-wide manner, i.e., without having to select a specific target region as for FISH. Application of this technique to large cohorts has demonstrated that large CNVs are associated with ID [8•], ASD [9, 10••, 11], and schizophrenia [12, 13] and that many of these CNVs are de novo mutations observed in the affected child and not in either parent. Furthermore, by identifying CNVs that are recurrent in multiple affected individuals new loci have been associated with neuropsychiatric disorders, including 1q21.1 [14], 16p11.2 [1518], 7q11.23 duplications [10••], and 17q12 deletions [19]. These recurrent loci are often caused by a process called non-allelic homologous recombination (NAHR) in which segmental duplications (large regions of very similar DNA) in close proximity lead to DNA replication errors resulting in deletion or duplication of the intervening unique DNA [20].

We will focus on deletion and duplication CNVs at four loci, 22q11.2, 15q11.2-13.1 (BP2-3), 16p11.2 (BP4-5), and 7q11.23 (Table 1), that show clear association with neuropsychiatric conditions and occur frequently enough to make a comparison between ID, ASD, schizophrenia, and unaffected controls.

Prevalence of CNVs by Disorder

To date these four CNVs have not been systematically assessed in a large birth cohort to determine the true population incidence and phenotypic spectrum. The prevalence in unaffected individuals has been estimated in a population of 101,655 individuals [21••], while the prevalence in individuals with neuropsychiatric disorders has been estimated in 3,955 individuals with idiopathic ASD [22], and 4,719 individuals with schizophrenia [23•]. No comparable cohort of ID cases has been assessed, therefore, we will rely on 8,772 individuals referred for microarray testing, predominantly due to ID with or without syndromic features [8•]. Due to the manner by which this ID cohort was ascertained the rate of CNVs is likely to be inflated, however, at present, it is the best dataset available. The observed prevalence of each deletion and duplication CNV at each of the four loci is shown for ID, ASD, schizophrenia, and controls in Fig. 1a; the corresponding odds ratios are shown in Table 2.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Prevalence of CNVs in three neuropsychiatric disorders and controls. a The prevalence of eight CNVs at four loci is shown for individuals with schizophrenia (Sch, n = 4,719), intellectual disability (ID, n = 8,772), autism spectrum disorder (ASD, n = 3,955), and in controls (n = 101,655). Some CNVs increase the risk of all three disorders (e.g., 16p11.2 duplications, 7q11.23 duplications) others show a degree of specificity being rarely observed in schizophrenia (16p11.2 deletions, 22q11.2 duplications, 15q11.2-13.1 duplications), or rarely observed in ASD and schizophrenia (7q11.23 deletions). b The expected pattern of CNV prevalence based on four models of etiology described in the main text. The "trunk and branches" model shows a pattern of asymmetric sharing that matches many of the CNVs in (a)

Table 2 Odds ratios and 95 % confidence intervals for the eight CNVs in three neuropsychiatric disorders compared with controls along with the etiological model that best fits the pattern

22q11.2: Three Syndromes Unified by a Genetic Diagnosis

Deletions at 22q11.2 are observed in about 1 in 6,000 births [24] and result in a wide variety of phenotypes, so much so that three separate syndromes were described. DiGeorge syndrome was described in 1967 based on a severe immune deficiency and absent thymus; hypocalcemia, hypoparathyroidism, and a right aortic arch were recorded in some infants. The authors noted that abnormal development of the third and fourth pharyngeal pouches could explain most of these features [25]. A decade later abnormalities of the heart and a distinctive facial appearance were described as cono-truncal anomaly face syndrome (CTAF) in Japan [26], while 2 years later velo-cardio-facial syndrome (VCFS) was described based on abnormalities of the palate (velum), heart, and face [27], it was also noted that the most of the children had learning disabilities. As mentioned earlier, the detection of a translocation that involved chromosome 22 [4] followed by deletions detected using high-resolution karyotyping and FISH allowed DiGeorge syndrome [28, 29], CTAF [30], and VCFS [31] to be recognized as sharing a common genetic cause: deletions at 22q11.2.

The deletion also leads to a wide spectrum of neurodevelopmental phenotypes, including ID, ASD, and schizophrenia [32] (Fig. 1a); of note, a lower IQ score appears to be associated with the increased risk of schizophrenia-spectrum psychosis [32].

Duplications at 22q11.2 are considerably more common being observed in about 1 in 1,300 adults [21••]. The phenotype is highly variable, including ID, speech impairment, psychomotor delay, growth retardation, hypotonia, and non-specific dysmorphic features [33]. It is frequently observed in individuals without neuropsychiatric diagnoses and is the least penetrant of the eight CNVs discussed in this review [21••]. It has recently been noted that the duplication was observed at a lower frequency in schizophrenia (0.014 %, 3 in 21,138 cases) than in unaffected controls (0.085 %, 23 in 27,133 controls) raising the possibility of imparting protection. An alternative interpretation is that, in the presence of other neuropsychiatric risk factors, 22q11.2 duplications tip the balance towards ID and ASD rather than schizophrenia.

15q11.2-13.1: Syndromes that Vary with Imprinting

Multiple segmental duplications are observed in the 15q11.2-13.1 region leading to five distinct "breakpoints" capable of generating recurrent large CNVs. We will focus only on the largest CNVs that include the region between breakpoint 2 and 3 (Table 1). In addition, the region between these two breakpoints is imprinted, which means that some of the genes within it are only expressed on the maternal or paternal chromosomes. Accordingly deletions give rise to two phenotypically distinct syndromes, Angelman and Prader–Willi, depending which parent’s chromosome the deletion is on.

A deletion on the maternal chromosome results in Angelman syndrome (AS), which is diagnosed in 1 in 10,000 to 1 in 20,000 live births [34, 35]. This syndrome was first described in 1965 [36] by the combination of distinctive behaviors and symptoms in three children. The disorder is characterized by severe ID with limited or absent speech, gait ataxia, seizures with a characteristic EEG pattern consisting of high voltage slow-wave activity, microcephaly, and hypopigmentation. Additionally, people with this condition exhibit an apparently happy disposition with excitability and inappropriate laughter; ASD is present in over 50 % of cases [37]. The association with 15q11.2-13.1 was detected through high-resolution karyotype analysis [38]. Analysis of an AS patient with a 15q inversion [39] and candidate gene mutation screening [40] determined that disruption of the ubiquitin ligase gene UBE3A within the 15q11.2-13.1 region was sufficient to cause AS. Only the maternal copy of UBE3A is expressed in the brain [41], though in the lymphoblasts and fibroblasts both copies are expressed.

Conversely, a deletion on the paternal chromosome leads to Prader–Willi syndrome (PWS), which is diagnosed in about 1 in 20,000 live births [42]. PWS was clinically described in nine patients with distinctive symptoms in 1965 [43]. It is characterized by neonatal hypotonia, low birth weight, and failure to thrive as an infant, mild ID, small hands and feet, hypopigmentation, and hypogonadism. As the individual enters early childhood, the failure to thrive is replaced by hyperphagia (excessive eating) that results in severe obesity [44]. In addition, these individuals have a distinct behavioral phenotype characterized by repetitive and stereotyped behaviors; up to 35 % of them receive a diagnosis of ASD [45]. The association with 15q11.2-13.1 was identified through high-resolution karyotype analysis [46] and the RNA processing gene SNURF-SNRPN or proximal small nucleolar RNAs (snoRNAs) are implicated in many symptoms [47].

Both AS and PWS can also arise through other processes including: (1) Uniparental disomy of chromosome 15, in which two chromosomes are present (as normal), however, they both come from the same parent; (2) Small mutations that disrupt the imprinting center on 15q11.2; and (3) Small mutations in UBE3A on the maternal chromosome leading to AS [48, 49] or in SNURF-SNRPN or snoRNAs on the paternal chromosome possibly leading to PWS [47, 49]. Of note, no cases of AS or PWS were identified in the ASD cohort (Fig. 1a); this is likely a consequence of the comparative rarity of these CNVs compared with other loci.

Duplications of 15q11.2-13.1 are observed in ~1 in 8,000 adults [21••]. These duplications were found in several cases of ASD [50] by karyotype analysis, but also in unaffected parents [51] raising doubt about the ASD association. The observation that duplications on the maternal chromosome carry considerably greater risk than those on the paternal chromosome [52] resolved this lack of segregation; maternal duplications are now recognized as one of the most frequent risk factors for ASD (Fig. 1a). While the features are less specific than for AS or PWS, a clear syndrome is characterized by ASD, ID, abnormal speech development, behavioral disturbances, seizures, hypotonia, short stature, and attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Concerningly, several cases of sudden death have been reported in individuals with 15q11.2-13.1 duplications [53] though the etiology of this phenomenon remains unknown. The duplication can occur by two mechanisms: (1) An additional supernumerary marker chromosome, also known as isodicentric chromosome 15 (IDIC15), with two extra copies leading to a total of four copies of the 15q11.2-13.1 region; and (2) An interstitial duplication leading to a total of three copies of the region.

16p11.2: A Syndrome Discovered by Genomics

CNVs at 16p11.2 are the most common of these four loci (Fig. 1a), however, the association with neuropsychiatric disorders was only demonstrated in 2008 [1517, 54]. The deletion is observed in 1 in 3,500 live births [55] and was initially described as a cause of idiopathic ASD. Subsequently, it has been identified in ID [56], schizophrenia [18], and a host of other neuropsychiatric conditions. As more individuals have been examined a syndrome has emerged including obesity, ID, speech problems, motor delays, and macrocephaly along with a variety of congenital abnormalities [57•]. The deletion is also observed in individuals without ID, ASD, or schizophrenia [21••]; this was initially described as incomplete penetrance, however, careful examination of carriers compared with non-carrier family controls showed a consistent decrease in IQ (by about two standard deviations) and social impairment (1.6 standard deviations on the SRS) [57•]. If all developmental and neuropsychiatric conditions are included in the definition of "affected" then 16p11.2 deletions are over 95 % penetrant [57•].

The 16p11.2 duplication is observed in 1 in 1,350 live births [55]. Like the 16p11.2 deletions, they were initially detected in individuals with idiopathic ASD [15, 17], but subsequently this CNV has been associated with many neurodevelopmental disorders including ID, speech delay, epilepsy, bipolar disorder, schizophrenia, ADHD, anxiety disorder, motor delay, and spastic quadriparesis [18, 56, 58]. The phenotype also includes low BMI, microcephaly, failure to thrive, congenital anomalies, and mild dysmorphic features [56].

While both the deletion and duplication increase risk for neuropsychiatric disorders, in particular ID, they display mirror phenotypes for head circumference and body mass index (BMI) with the combination of high BMI and macrocephaly in the deletion, and low BMI and microcephaly in the duplication [56, 59]. These relationships raise the possibility that these phenotypes are directly correlated with 16p11.2 gene dosage and possibly expression of specific genes.

7q11.23: A Genetic Locus for Human Sociability

Deletions at 7q11.23 cause Williams–Beuren syndrome (WBS) which was described in the early 1960′s [60, 61] and is characterized by supravalvular aortic stenosis, ID (with verbal IQ comparatively spared), hypersociable personality, growth retardation, endocrine abnormalities, and a distinctive facial appearance [62]. The causal role of 7q11.23 deletions was discovered through FISH analysis of the Elastin (ELN) gene in WBS patients following the recognition of ELN disruption as a cause of familial supravalvular aortic stenosis [63]. The disorder affects about 1 in 10,000 individuals [62]. Individuals with WBS can have a concurrent diagnosis of ASD [64], however, this is uncommon [62].

While the phenotype of deletions at 7q11.23 was recognized as a distinct syndrome over 50 years ago, the first description of a phenotype associated with duplications at this locus was in 2005 [65]. Analysis of multiple cases has revealed a new syndrome characterized by severe speech delay, mild ID, and non-specific dysmorphic features. Of note, the individuals tended to exhibit poor eye contact and to be withdrawn [66, 67]. The population frequency is estimated to be similar to that for WBS, affecting 1 in 13,000–1 in 20,000 individuals [67]. Duplications at 7q11.23 have been associated with ASD [19] and schizophrenia [68].

In a similar manner to 16p11.2 CNVs, both the deletion and duplication CNVs at 7q11.23 have an adverse effect on an individual’s IQ, but a mirror phenotype is seen with sociability and speech. Sociability is increased and verbal IQ is relatively preserved in the deletion, while sociability is reduced and there is severe speech impairment in the duplication. Again this raises the possibility of direct correlation between these phenotypes and gene dosage/expression.

Multiple Risk Factors

These four CNV loci are among the most common risk factors for neuropsychiatric disorders, however, together they account for <2.5 % of patients with these disorders. In ASD alone it is estimated that up to 1,000 distinct genetic risk factors may contribute to the phenotype [69, 70], and there is no reason to expect radically different figures in ID or schizophrenia.

Despite being treated as single diagnostic entities, each affected individual is likely to have a unique combination of risk factors. In ASD, this etiological complexity has led to the term “the autisms” [71] to reflect this extensive phenotypic and genetic heterogeneity contained in the single diagnostic label.

However, aspects of the phenotype are sufficiently similar to allow the use of ID, ASD, and schizophrenia as diagnoses, therefore, the myriad risk factors must converge on a limited number of pathophysiological mechanisms that cause the core features of each condition. Several groups have attempted to leverage this convergence by seeking stages of brain development and anatomical regions of the brain at which known genetic risk factors are expressed. Such convergence has been observed in the pre-frontal cortex during mid-fetal development in both ASD [7274] and schizophrenia [75]. These approaches show the potential for rigorously identified genetic risk factors to be leveraged to reveal fundamental insights into the pathology of neuropsychiatric disorders.

Penetrance in Neuropsychiatric Traits

These syndromes challenge the conventional notion of penetrance. While 16p11.2 deletions show incomplete penetrance for ID and ASD (when viewed as dichotomous, qualitative traits) they appear to cause a significant deleterious impact in cognition and social behavior, as measured by IQ and the Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS) (both of which are continuous, quantitative traits), in both cases and apparently unaffected carriers [57•]. While the disorder is incompletely penetrant, the effect is completely penetrant. Similarly when unaffected carriers discovered in a population-scale search for CNVs were examined, they displayed a higher level of cognitive deficits than individuals without such CNVs [21••]. This concept of penetrance across multiple neurodevelopmental and neuropsychiatric disorders and the use of continuous, quantitative measures such as SRS and IQ to compare probands with their unaffected first-degree relatives is described further in the Developmental Brain Dysfunction model [76••].

Phenotypic Overlap

One of the most unexpected and striking findings from analysis of CNVs in neuropsychiatric disorders has been the extent to which specific loci are shared between disorders. For example, deletions at both 22q11.2 (DiGeorge syndrome) and 16p11.2 increase the individual’s risk for ID [6, 77], ASD [10••, 78••], and schizophrenia [6, 23•]. Furthermore, while the presence of these CNVs in ASD cohorts does not correlate with ASD severity, they do correlate (weakly) with IQ [10••]. These observations challenge the notion that ID, ASD, and schizophrenia are three entirely distinct diagnostic and etiological categories. We will consider whether the CNV data support or contradict four models for the relationship between neuropsychiatric phenotype and etiological mechanism.

Diagnostic Unity

The simplest explanation is that these three neuropsychiatric disorders are, in fact, a single etiological entity. Clinical observations would argue against this; a diagnosis of ASD is rarely converted to a diagnosis of schizophrenia [79], which holds true in a reciprocal fashion. Similarly individuals with ASD or schizophrenia can have a high IQ. The genetic evidence is also inconsistent with this explanation. We would expect each CNV to be equally prevalent in all three conditions (Fig. 1b), however, with the exception of 16p11.2 duplications, this does not appear to be the case (Fig. 1a).

IQ Buffering in ASD and Schizophrenia

Given the consistency with which de novo mutations are associated with ID (Fig. 1a) and the correlation of de novo mutations with IQ in ASD [10••] and, possibly, schizophrenia [80] it could be argued that the primary effect of de novo CNVs is to reduce IQ leading to ID directly and ASD and schizophrenia indirectly. In this model [81] limited social skills or the tendency to disordered thought patterns are distributed normally within the population, but a higher IQ has a buffering effect that masks the phenotype. A de novo CNV disrupts this buffering effect potentially leading to ASD or schizophrenia. Under this model, we would expect each de novo CNV to be most prevalent in ID and for ASD and schizophrenia to mirror this risk (Fig. 1b) however, no CNVs show this pattern clearly.

IQ Buffering in ASD only

This is the same concept as the last model, however, given the closer relationship between IQ and ASD than between IQ and schizophrenia we will consider the IQ buffering effect to only relate to ASD. At least two CNVs appear to show a consistent pattern (16p11.2 deletions, 7q11.23 duplications), however, the majority of CNVs show a more complex pattern (Fig. 1a).

Trunk and Branches

De novo CNVs can disrupt key processes in brain development (the trunk), thus raising the risk for multiple neuropsychiatric disorders [76••], but they can also disrupt more specific neurological processes (the branches), e.g., social skills, that further increase the risk for specific subsets of neuropsychiatric disorders. Consequently, de novo CNVs show a pattern of asymmetric sharing in which they are observed in multiple neuropsychiatric disorders, but are particularly prevalent in a subset of disorders. Most CNVs show this asymmetric sharing: 15q11.2-13.1 duplications disproportionately increase the risk of ASD, 22q11.2 deletions increase the schizophrenia risk more than ASD risk, and 22q11.2 duplications predominantly increase ID risk and may even be protective against schizophrenia [82].

Therefore, while some CNVs increase the general risk of neuropsychiatric disorders, the majority appear to increase the risk of certain conditions more than others, suggesting that at least some genetic risk factors may be specific to each disorder. These CNV risk factors not only modify an individual’s position on continuous neuropsychiatric traits but also do so in an asymmetric manner, affecting some traits to a greater extent than others. The extent of this sharing between phenotypes may reflect the degree to which etiological mechanisms are shared between disorders.

"Missed Syndromes"

While some of these CNVs cause well-recognized syndromes (DiGeorge, WBS, AS, PWS) others, such as 16p11.2 and 7q11.23 duplications, were discovered in cases of idiopathic ASD or schizophrenia without apparently syndromic features. It was only with the examination of large cohorts of individuals that share the same CNV that a syndrome has emerged.

This raises a question regarding which factors separate recognizable syndromes from those that are "missed." The clearest factor would appear to be the presence of distinctive, uncommon, and specific characteristics. For example, WBS accounts for a substantial proportion of individuals with supravalvular aortic stenosis and is associated with a distinctive appearance and behaviors. Similarly the distinctive combination of absent thymus and hypoparathyroidism allowed the recognition of DiGeorge syndrome [25]. Conversely, the abnormalities in cognition, behavior, and energy balance observed in CNVs at 16p11.2 are observed relatively frequently in the population, such that large cohorts of cases are required to demonstrate syndromic features [57•, 59].

Population frequency of the syndrome must play a role, since several cases must be brought to the attention of the clinician and/or researcher, however, above this threshold population frequency would appear to be less important than distinctive features, since 16p11.2 is three times more common than 15q11.2-13.1 but was recognized considerably later. Penetrance might also be expected to play a role, since a highly penetrant variant will show a clear segregation with the disorder allowing easier recognition. While the late discovery of syndromes for 16p11.2 and 22q11.2 duplications would support this, 7q11.23 duplications have the highest penetrance of all eight CNVs (Table 1), but were discovered almost last.

In summary, a syndrome has emerged for CNVs at all four loci associated with neuropsychiatric disorders after sufficient cases have been examined, however, only syndromes with particularly distinctive features are identified prior to genomic analysis of large cohorts.

Single Gene Mutations

Exome sequencing in ID [83, 84], ASD [69, 8587], and schizophrenia [80] has highlighted the role of de novo loss of function mutations as risk factors for these disorders. Furthermore, by observing multiple mutations in the same gene in different individuals, specific genes have been associated with disorders [69].

As with de novo CNVs, no single gene will explain a large proportion of ID, ASD, or schizophrenia; FMR1 triplet expansions (Fragile X) are the most common single gene cause of ID [88] and ASD [89, 90] accounting for only 4 % and 1 % of cases, respectively. Unlike the CNVs, most of the identified genes have no equivalent mutations in controls suggesting 100 % penetrance, however, with increasing sample size this is likely to change.

It is less clear whether single gene mutations will show similar patterns of asymmetric sharing between disorders as is observed with CNVs (Fig. 1a). This mixture of phenotypic effects may be a consequence of the multiple genes within a CNV; if this were the case, we might expect to see a group of genes that increase the general risk of neuropsychiatric disorders (disrupting the "trunk") and another group that are specific to one disorder (disrupting the "branches"). Alternatively the pleiotropic effects of each gene might lead to similarly diverse patterns of prevalence within these disorders as is seen with CNVs.

Finally, it is unclear whether careful examination of individuals with disruption of the same gene will lead to a recognizable syndrome or not. Some of the syndromic features appear to be a consequence of the multiple genes within each CNV. However, disruption of a single gene can certainly cause syndromic effects (e.g., FMR1, TSC1); it remains to be seen if these genes represent the exception or the rule.

Conclusion

Advances in technology have allowed systematic identification of CNVs that mediate risk for neuropsychiatric disorders leading to the recognition of novel syndromes (16p11.2, 7q11.23 duplications). The identification of these CNVs has transformed our understanding of the genomic architecture of these disorders leading to the conclusion that thousands of risk factors converge on a limited number of diagnoses. Furthermore, a complex pattern of asymmetric sharing of genetic risk factors between neuropsychiatric disorders suggests that ID, ASD, and schizophrenia are caused by a combination of shared and distinct neuropathological mechanisms.