Skip to main content
Log in

Before Cumulative Culture

The Evolutionary Origins of Overimitation and Shared Intentionality

  • Published:
Human Nature Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In the 7 million years or so since humans shared a common ancestor with chimpanzees we have colonized more of the planet’s terrestrial habitat than any other mammalian species and come to account for more biomass than all other terrestrial vertebrates combined. Chimpanzees, in contrast to and under pressure from ourselves, have veered toward extinction. There are multiple reasons for the stark evolutionary trajectories humans and chimpanzees have taken. Recent theoretical and empirical interest has focused on the emergence of cumulative culture whereby technological innovations are progressively incorporated into a population’s stock of skills and knowledge, generating ever more sophisticated repertoires. Here we look at the role of high-fidelity imitation and intention-reading in the establishment of cumulative culture. By focusing on the lithic record, we aim to identify when in our evolutionary history these skills became part of our ancestors’ behavioral repertoire. We argue that evidence of cooperative construction in stone tool manufacture, along with speculation regarding changes to the mirror neurone system, hint at the foundations of overimitation and shared intentionality around 2 million years ago. However, these are not the only ingredients of cumulative culture, which is why we do not see convincing evidence for it until slightly more than a million years later.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Alperson-Afil, N., Sharon, G., Kislev, M., Melamed, Y., Zohar, I., Ashkenazi, S., & Hartman, G. (2009). Spatial organization of hominin activities at Gesher Benot Ya’aqov, Israel. Science, 326(5960), 1677–1680.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beyene, Y., Katoh, S., WoldeGabriel, G., Hart, W. K., Uto, K., Sudo, M., & Suwa, G. (2013). The characteristics and chronology of the earliest Acheulean at Konso, Ethiopia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(5), 1584–1591.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boesch, C. (2002). Cooperative hunting roles among Tai chimpanzees. Human Nature, 13(1), 27–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bonnie, K. E., Horner, V., Whiten, A., & de Waal, F. B. (2007). Spread of arbitrary conventions among chimpanzees: a controlled experiment. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 274(1608), 367–372.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., and Richerson, P.J. (1996). Why culture is common, but cultural evolution is rare. Proceedings of the British Academy, 88, 77–93.

  • Boyd, R., Richerson, P. J., & Henrich, J. (2011). The cultural niche: why social learning is essential for human adaptation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(Supplement 2), 10918–10925.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bushnell, E., Sidman, J., & Brugger, A. (2006). Transfer according to the means in human infants: the secret to generative tool-use. In B. Blandine & R. Valentine (Eds.), Stone knapping: the necessary conditions for a uniquely hominid behaviour (pp. 303–317). Cambridge: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buttelmann, D., Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Rational tool use and tool choice in human infants and great apes. Child Development, 79(3), 609–626.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Call, J., Hare, B., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2004). ‘Unwilling’versus ‘unable’: chimpanzees’ understanding of human intentional action. Developmental Science, 7(4), 488–498.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention and imitative learning in children, chimpanzees, and enculturated chimpanzees. Social Development, 4(3), 217–237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caruana, M. V., d’Errico, F., & Backwell, L. (2013). Early hominin social learning strategies underlying the use and production of bone and stone tools. In C. Sanz, J. Call, & C. Boesch (Eds.), Tool use in animals: cognition and ecology (pp. 242–285). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Corvinus, G. (1983). A survey of the Pravara river system in western Maharashtra, India: the excavations of the Acheulian site of Chirki-on-Pravara, 2 vols. Tübingen: Tübinger Monographien zur Urgeschichte, 7.

  • Davidson, I. (2002). The ‘finished artefact fallacy’: Acheulean handaxes and language origins. In A. Wray (Ed.), The transition to language (pp. 180–203). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • de la Torre, I. (2004). The Omo revisited: Evaluating the technological skills of Pliocene hominids. Current Anthropology, 45(4), 439–465.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Falk, D. (1987). Hominid paleoneurology. Annual Review of Anthropology, 16, 13–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferraro, J. V., Plummer, T. W., Pobiner, B. L., Oliver, J. S., Bishop, L. C., Braun, D. R., & Seaman, J. W., Jr. (2013). Earliest archaeological evidence of persistent hominin carnivory. PLoS One, 8(4), e62174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fitch, F. J., Miller, J. A., & Mitchell, J. G. (1996). Current events: dating of the KBS tuff and Homo rudolfensis. Journal of Human Evolution, 30(3), 277–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flynn, E., & Smith, K. (2012). Investigating the mechanisms of cultural acquisition: how pervasive is overimitation in adults? Social Psychology, 43(4), 185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fogassi, L., Ferrari, P. F., Gesierich, B., Rozzi, S., Chersi, F., & Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Parietal lobe: from action organization to intention understanding. Science, 308(5722), 662–667.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fuhrmann, D., Ravignani, A., Marshall-Pescini, S., & Whiten, A. (2014). Synchrony and motor mimicking in chimpanzee observational learning. Scientific Reports, 4, 5283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain, 119(2), 593–609.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V., Keysers, C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2004). A unifying view of the basis of social cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(9), 396–403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V., Rochat, M., Cossu, G., & Sinigaglia, C. (2009). Motor cognition and its role in the phylogeny and ontogeny of action understanding. Developmental Psychology, 45(1), 103–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gergely, G., & Csibra, G. (2006). Sylvia’s recipe: the role of imitation and pedagogy in the transmission of cultural knowledge. In N. J. Enfield & S. C. Levinson (Eds.), Roots of human sociality: Culture, cognition, and human interaction (pp. 229–255). Oxford: Berg.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gergely, G., Nádasdy, Z., Csibra, G., & Biro, S. (1995). Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age. Cognition, 56(2), 165–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goren-Inbar, N. (2011). Culture and cognition in the Acheulian industry: a case study from Gesher Benot Ya’aqov. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B: Biological Sciences, 366(1567), 1038–1049.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goren-Inbar, N., Lister, A., Werker, E., & Chech, M. (1994). A butchered elephant skull and associated artifacts from the Acheulian site of Gesher Benot Ya’aqov, Israel. Paléorient, 20, 99–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grezes, J., Costes, N., & Decety, J. (1998). Top down effect of strategy on the perception of human biological motion: a PET investigation.. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 15, 553–582.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holloway, R. L. (1983). Human paleontological evidence relevant to language behavior. Human Neurobiology, 2(3), 105–114.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holloway, R. L., Broadfield, D. C., Yuan, M. S., Schwartz, J. H., & Tattersall, I. (2004). The human fossil record (vol. 3): Brain endocasts—The paleoneurological evidence. Hoboken: Wiley.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hopkinson, T., Nowell, A., & White, M. (2013). Life histories, metapopulation ecology, and innovation in the Acheulian. PaleoAnthropology, 2013, 61–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horner, V., & Whiten, A. (2005). Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens). Animal Cognition, 8(3), 164–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iacoboni, M. (2009). Imitation, empathy, and mirror neurons. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 653–670.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iacoboni, M., & Dapretto, M. (2006). The mirror neuron system and the consequences of its dysfunction. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7(12), 942–951.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iacoboni, M., Woods, R. P., Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Mazziotta, J. C., & Rizzolatti, G. (1999). Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. Science, 286(5449), 2526–2528.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iacoboni, M., Molnar-Szakacs, I., Gallese, V., Buccino, G., Mazziotta, J. C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Grasping the intentions of others with one’s own mirror neuron system. PLoS Biology, 3(3), e79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kempe, M., & Mesoudi, A. (2014). An experimental demonstration of the effect of group size on cultural accumulation. Evolution and Human Behavior, 35(4), 285–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kempe, M., Lycett, S., & Mesoudi, A. (2012). An experimental test of the accumulated copying error model of cultural mutation for Acheulean handaxe size. PLoS One, 7(11), e48333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kenward, B. (2012). Over-imitating preschoolers believe unnecessary actions are normative and enforce their performance by a third party. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112, 195–207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keupp, S., Behne, T., & Rakoczy, H. (2013). Why do children overimitate? Normativity is crucial. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116, 392–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kilner, J. M., Friston, K. J., & Frith, C. D. (2007). Predictive coding: an account of the mirror neuron system. Cognitive Processing, 8(3), 159–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kohn, M., & Mithen, S. (1999). Handaxes: products of sexual selection? Antiquity, 73, 518–526.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lemorini, C., Plummer, T. W., Braun, D. R., Crittenden, A. N., Ditchfield, P. W., Bishop, L. C., & Schoeninger, M. J. (2014). Old stones’ song: use-wear experiments and analysis of the Oldowan quartz and quartzite assemblage from Kanjera South (Kenya). Journal of Human Evolution, 72, 10–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lepre, C. J., Roche, H., Kent, D. V., Harmand, S., Quinn, R. L., Brugal, J.-P., & Feibel, C. S. (2011). An earlier origin for the Acheulian. Nature, 477(7362), 82–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, K. R., Johnson-Frey, S. H., & Grafton, S. T. (2004). Functional imaging of face and hand imitation: towards a motor theory of empathy. NeuroImage, 21(2), 601–607.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., Henning, A., Striano, T., & Tomasello, M. (2004). Twelve‐month‐olds point to share attention and interest. Developmental Science, 7(3), 297–307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lycett, S. J. (2008). Acheulean variation and selection: does handaxe symmetry fit neutral expectations? Journal of Archaeological Science, 35(9), 2640–2648.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lycett, S. J., & Gowlett, J. A. (2008). On questions surrounding the Acheulean ‘tradition’. World Archaeology, 40(3), 295–315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lyons, D. E., Young, A. G., & Keil, F. C. (2007). The hidden structure of overimitation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 19751–19756.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Machin, A. J., Hosfield, R. T., & Mithen, S. J. (2007). Why are some handaxes symmetrical? Testing the influence of handaxe morphology on butchery effectiveness. Journal of Archaeological Science, 34(6), 883–893.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marsh, L., Ropar, D., & Hamilton, A. (2014). The social modulation of imitation fidelity in school-age children. PloS One, 9, e86127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGuigan, N., Whiten, A., Flynn, E., & Horner, V. (2007). Imitation of causally opaque vesus causally transparent tool use by 3- and 5-year-old children. Cognitive Development, 22, 353–364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N. (1995). Understanding the intentions of others: re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology, 31(5), 838–850.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Molenberghs, P., Cunnington, R., & Mattingley, J. B. (2012). Brain regions with mirror properties: a meta-analysis of 125 human fMRI studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(1), 341–349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, M. W. (2003). Australian Aboriginal biface reduction techniques on the Georgina River, Camooweal, Queensland. Australian Archaeology, 56, 22–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mukamel, R., Ekstrom, A. D., Kaplan, J., Iacoboni, M., & Fried, I. (2010). Single-neuron responses in humans during execution and observation of actions. Current Biology, 20(8), 750–756.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nielsen, M. (2006). Copying actions and copying outcomes: social learning through the second year. Developmental Psychology, 42, 555–565.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nielsen, M. (2012). Imitation, pretend play and childhood: essential elements in the evolution of human culture? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126, 170–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nielsen, M., & Blank, C. (2011). Imitation in young children: when who gets copied is more important than what gets copied. Developmental Psychology, 47, 1050–1053.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nielsen, M., Moore, C., & Mohamedally, J. (2012). Young children overimitate in third-party contexts. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112(1), 73–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nielsen, M., Kapitány, R., & Elkins, R. (2015). The perpetuation of ritualistic actions as revealed by young children’s transmission of normative behavior. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36, 191–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Over, H., & Carpenter, M. (2012). Putting the social into social learning: explaining both selectivity and fidelity in children’s copying behavior. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126, 182–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petraglia, M., LaPorta, P., & Paddayya, K. (1999). The first Acheulian quarry in India: stone tool manufacture, biface morphology, and behaviors. Journal of Anthropological Research, 55, 39–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piperno, M., & Tagliacozzo, A. (2001). The elephant butchery area at the Middle Pleistocene site of Notarchirico (Venosa, Basilicata, Italy). In G. Cavarretta, P. Gioia, M. Mussi, & M. R. Palombo (Eds.), La terra degli elefanti/The world of elephants (pp. 230–236). Rome: Consiglio Nazionale delle Ricerche.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pitts, M. W., & Roberts, M. (1997). Fairweather Eden: life in Britain half a million years ago as revealed by the excavations at Boxgrove. London: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rabinovich, R., Gaudzinski-Windheuser, S., & Goren-Inbar, N. (2008). Systematic butchering of fallow deer (Dama) at the early Middle Pleistocene Acheulian site of Gesher Benot Ya’aqov (Israel). Journal of Human Evolution, 54(1), 134–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rein, R., Bril, B., & Nonaka, T. (2013). Coordination strategies used in stone knapping. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 150(4), 539–550.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neurosciences, 27, 169–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Gallese, V., & Fogassi, L. (1996). Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Cognitive Brain Research, 3(2), 131–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roche, H., Delagnes, A., Brugal, J.-P., Feibel, C., Kibunjia, M., Mourre, V., & Texier, P.-J. (1999). Early hominid stone tool production and technical skill 2.34 myr ago in West Turkana, Kenya. Nature, 399(6731), 57–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roux, V., & Bril, B. (Eds.). (2005). Stone knapping: the necessary conditions for a uniquely hominin behaviour. Cambridge: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sangode, S., Mishra, S., Naik, S., & Deo, S. (2007). Magnetostratigraphy of the Quaternary sediments associated with some Toba tephra and Acheulian artefact bearing localities in the western and central India. Gondwana Magazine, 10, 111–121.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sanz, C., Call, J., & Morgan, D. (2009). Design complexity in termite-fishing tools of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Biology Letters, 5(3), 293–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scaife, M., & Bruner, J. S. (1975). The capacity for joint visual attention in the infant. Nature, 253, 265–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sharon, G., Alperson-Afil, N., & Goren-Inbar, N. (2011). Cultural conservatism and variability in the Acheulian sequence of Gesher Benot Ya’aqov. Journal of Human Evolution, 60(4), 387–397.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shipton, C. (2013). A million years of hominin sociality and cognition: Acheulean bifaces in the Hunsgi-Baichbal Valley, India. Oxford: Archaeopress.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shipton, C., Petraglia, M., & Paddayya, K. (2009). Stone tool experiments and reduction methods at the Acheulean site of Isampur Quarry, India. Antiquity, 83(321), 769–785.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shipton, C., Clarkson, C., Pal, J., Jones, S., Roberts, R., Harris, C., & Petraglia, M. (2013). Generativity, hierarchical action and recursion in the technology of the Acheulean to Middle Palaeolithic transition: a perspective from Patpara, the Son Valley, India. Journal of Human Evolution, 65, 93–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stiner, M. C., Barkai, R., & Gopher, A. (2009). Cooperative hunting and meat sharing 400–200 kya at Qesem Cave, Israel. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(32), 13207–13212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stout, D., Schick, K., & Toth, N. (2009). Understanding Oldowan knapping skill: an experimental study of skill acquisition in modern humans. In K. Schick & N. Toth (Eds.), The cutting edge: new approaches to the archaeology of human origins (pp. 247–265). Gosport: Stone Age Institute Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stout, D., Semaw, S., Rogers, M. J., & Cauche, D. (2010). Technological variation in the earliest Oldowan from Gona, Afar, Ethiopia. Journal of Human Evolution, 58(6), 474–491.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stout, D., Passingham, R., Frith, C., Apel, J., & Chaminade, T. (2011). Technology, expertise and social cognition in human evolution. European Journal of Neuroscience, 33(7), 1328–1338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tennie, C., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Ratcheting up the ratchet: on the evolution of cumulative culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B: Biological Sciences, 364(1528), 2405–2415.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tennie, C., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Untrained chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) fail to imitate novel actions. PLoS One, 7(8), e41548.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tennie, C., Braun, D. R., & McPherron, S. P. (2015). The island test for cumulative culture in Paleolithic cultures. New York: Springer Nature.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tixier, J., & Inizian, M.-L. (1983). Préhistoire de la pierre taillée. 1. Terminologie et technologie. France: Valbonne.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tobias, P. V. (1987). The brain of Homo habilis: a new level of organization in cerebral evolution. Journal of Human Evolution, 16(7), 741–761.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (1999). The human adaptation for culture. Annual Review of Anthropology, 28, 509–529.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., & Moll, H. (2010). The gap is social: human shared intentionality and culture. In P. Kappeler & J. Silk (Eds.), Mind the gap (pp. 331–349). Berlin: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: the origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(05), 675–691.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., Melis, A. P., Tennie, C., Wyman, E., & Herrmann, E. (2012). Two key steps in the evolution of human cooperation. Current Anthropology, 53(6), 673–692.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toth, N. (1985). The Oldowan reassessed: a close look at early stone artifacts. Journal of Archaeological Science, 12(2), 101–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toth, N., Schick, K., & Semaw, S. (2006). A comparative study of the stone tool-making skills of Pan, Australopithecus, and Homo sapiens. In N. Toth & K. Schick (Eds.), The Oldowan: case studies into the earliest Stone Age (pp. 155–222). Gosport: Stone Age Institute Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Villa, P. (1990). Torralba and Aridos: elephant exploitation in Middle Pleistocene Spain. Journal of Human Evolution, 19(3), 299–309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walker, A., & Leakey, R. E. (1993). The Nariokotome Homo erectus skeleton. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Wasielewski, H. (2014). Imitation is necessary for cumulative cultural evolution in an unfamiliar, opaque task. Human Nature, 25(1), 161–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whiten, A., Horner, V., & Marshall-Pescini, S. (2003). Cultural panthropology. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 12(2), 92–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woodward, A. L. (1999). Infants’ ability to distinguish between purposeful and non-purposeful behaviors. Infant Behavior & Development, 22(2), 145–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wynn, T. (2002). Archaeology and cognitive evolution. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25(03), 389–402.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yang, D., Sidman, J., & Bushnell, E. W. (2010). Beyond the information given: infants’ transfer of actions learned through imitation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 106(1), 62–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yang, D. J., Bushnell, E. W., Buchanan, D. W., & Sobel, D. M. (2013). Infants’ use of contextual cues in the generalization of effective actions from imitation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116(2), 510–531.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank Professors K. Paddayya, Sheila Mishra, and J. N. Pal and the Archaeological Survey of India for access to the archaeological collections discussed and illustrated in this paper. We thank three anonymous reviewers for their comments on an earlier draft of this paper.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Ceri Shipton or Mark Nielsen.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Shipton, C., Nielsen, M. Before Cumulative Culture. Hum Nat 26, 331–345 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-015-9233-8

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-015-9233-8

Keywords

Navigation