Skip to main content
Log in

What Aspects of Society Matter for the Quality of Life of a Minority? Global Evidence from the New Gay Happiness Index

  • Published:
Social Indicators Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

There is great variation in views on and treatment of minorities such as gay men across the world. We are the first to pinpoint what features of societies are beneficial to gay men’s quality of life by using a unique new cross-country dataset covering 110 countries, the Gay Happiness Index. It covers how gays perceive public opinion about them, how they experience behavior towards them and how satisfied they are with their lives. Our study is based on the premise that it is important to look at minority-specific effects of policies and institutions and not solely at the effects for the average citizen, as well as the transmission mechanisms through which policies and institutions affect life satisfaction. We find that factors such as equal legal rights for gay people, GDP per capita, democracy and globalization relate positively to the quality of life of gay men, primarily by shaping public opinion and behavior in a pro-gay direction. Religion (the shares of Muslims and Orthodox Christians) and living in a post-communist country tend to relate negatively to our quality of life indicators. Most of these factors have been shown to matter for the well-being of people in general as well, which may be taken to suggest that gay people benefit from being included in society—legally, socially and economically—on the same terms as others.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1

Source: World Values Survey (2015)

Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Rawls’s basic idea is that people will be able to choose principles of justice that are fair in an original position characterized in the following way: “Among the essential features of this situation is that no one knows his place in society, his class position or social status, nor does any one know his fortune in the distribution of natural assets and abilities, his intelligence, strength and the like. We shall even assume that the parties do not know their conceptions of the good or their special psychological propensities. The principles of justice are chosen behind a veil of ignorance.” (Rawls 1971: 12). The veil of ignorance can thus help us consider principles or institutions embodying such principles without knowing how they would affect us. As an example, when considering same-sex marriage, we would have to disregard whether we are gay, fundamentalist Christian or anything else.

  2. According to the American Psychological Association (2008), “[s]exual orientation refers to an enduring pattern of emotional, romantic, and/or sexual attractions to men, women, or both sexes”, and by “gays” we mean those men with a sexual orientation that entails exclusive attraction to other men. The results of this study may well extend to other minority groups than gays. In fact, Inglehart and Abramson (1999) argue that inclusiveness toward gay men is a useful indicator of tolerant attitudes overall.

  3. On the history of attitudes towards homosexuality, see e.g., Duberman et al. (1990) or Mondimore (1996).

  4. On the rise of sexual freedom more generally around the world, see Alexander et al. (in press). Yet, these developments do not mean that there is no de facto discrimination of gays also in Western countries—see, e.g., Ahmed et al. (2013), Boeri et al. (2015), Hammarstedt et al. (2015) and Patacchini et al. (2015).

  5. We use the term “quality of life” rather than “gay happiness” since the index is comprised of factors that are not, as such, all measures of happiness (even though they may affect happiness).

  6. On the policy relevance of life satisfaction findings, with a special argumentation for how they matter for the design of institutions, see Frey and Stutzer (2012).

  7. For more on individual-level factors of importance to the quality of life of gay people, see van den Akker et al. (2013) and Powdthavee and Wooden (2014). Nussbaum and Sen (1993) discuss what constitutes quality of life, while Bjørnskov et al. (2008), Dolan et al. (2008) and Frey and Stutzer (2002, 2012) provide overviews of the life-satisfaction literature.

  8. While Clark et al. (2008: 123) state that “greater economic prosperity at some point ceases to buy more happiness”, recent studies, such as Deaton (2008) and Stevenson and Wolfers (2008), find that the well-being–income relationship is roughly a linear-log relationship and that there is no evidence of satiation.

  9. As discussed by Whitam (1983), homosexuality was seen as a symptom of bourgeois decadence in the Soviet Union and was therefore seen as something to be eradicated. As a number of other cultural and ideological factors have tended to persist after the post-communist transition, a generally negative view of homosexuality may be part of a communist legacy (Heineck and Süssmuth 2013; Necker and Voskort 2014).

  10. A strength of the sample is that it is large and drawn from a substantial group of very diverse countries, and while it is based on an online survey, there are indications of smaller and smaller differences between offline and online survey populations as Internet access spreads (Fricker and Schonlau 2002). It bears noting, for example, that the mean age of respondents in this online survey is similar to that found in other surveys (see e.g., Powdthavee and Wooden 2014). Nevertheless, there might still be a concern that the sample is non-representative (Wilson and Laskey 2003). Let us address such a concern with four points. First, we deal with this in Sect. 4, by weighting the sample such that observations from countries with fewer than 100 respondents are downplayed, by carrying out an outlier analysis in the form of a jackknife exercise and by separating estimates for rich and poor countries. The inclusion of regional dummies also alleviates part of the concern to the extent that they capture broad factors affecting survey participation. Second, even if skewedness exists, it may be reasonable to assume that it is similar across countries, which makes comparisons of our kind less problematic. Third, it may be the case that online surveys are actually more representative of gay men in general than traditional surveys that ask gay men who are members of particular organizations or who visit particular physical venues, since we believe that this group is more prone, in most countries and not least in countries where there is legal and social disapprobation, to socialize via the web compared to people in general. As noted in Tikkanen and Ross (2003), one difference between men who visit gay chat rooms online and those who never do is that the former group has less often publicly come out, indicating that an Internet survey of this kind may comprise people that would otherwise not be survey participants. Fourth, Planet Romeo can be accessed via smartphones, both on the web and through an app, which does not make it necessary to own a computer to participate in the survey; it is not least noteworthy that increasingly, people in developing countries use phones for financial transactions and social interaction. All this being said, it should be noted that our results mainly apply to gay men who use the Internet in some form.

  11. To some extent, the critique hides an important but never stated assumption. If dissatisfaction is to result in a desire for specific behavior, such as moving to another location, dissatisfied respondents must also perceive that it is possible to do so. If moving is not perceived to be a practical possibility—either because people are somehow barred from moving or if respondents believe that the situation is equally bad at alternative locations—it is entirely possible to be strongly dissatisfied without having incentives to change behavior.

  12. We use PCA as a way to inform us about how to create indices that maximize variation while at the same time avoiding testing highly correlated indices against each other. The particular attraction with PCA is that the number of components and the separation of variables into indices are entirely driven by the data structure and thus free of any theoretical or normative priors. While factor analysis (FA) could have been used as the main method, it is more suitable if one wishes to test a theoretical model of latent factors causing observed variables, while our interest lies in reducing a set of correlated observed variables to a smaller set of independent composite variables, which is done through the PCA. The choice of PCA is also supported by Cronbach’s Alpha (see Tables 1, 2). Yet, our experience is that the use of related types of analysis in general provides support for the same division of the variables. Other similar techniques such as FA that do not necessarily generate orthogonal solutions nevertheless tend to result in similar solutions as those we find here using PCA. And we have, as a result of this comment, run a FA as well, which reassuringly gives very similar results; not least, both PCA and FA suggest that the original GHI sub-index of public behavior hides several dimensions, which is why we use the three new indices reflecting public behavior.

  13. Although one might criticize the choice of equal weights of variables in the indices, since the loadings in some cases seem different, a full set of tests suggests that no loadings differ significantly within each index. We therefore prefer a choice of equal weights in order to provide maximally transparent indices and results.

  14. We note that while we use the term favorable opinion, the opinions of the surrounding society need not be favorable in an absolute sense. We merely require that they are not less favorable than the opinions of other citizens with different sexual preferences. As such, we treat normative neutrality as representing a favorable opinion.

  15. The original GHI contains 25 components organized into three sub-indices. However, we only use 24 of the components—component 8 of the public opinion sub-index (which covers respondents’ ratings of their environment more gay-friendly than anti-gay based on the “Perception of Stigma Scale”) has been excluded. The reason is that this question is hard to interpret on the basis of being an average of nine underlying questions and that preliminary statistical analysis indicates that the three separate replies (friendly, un-friendly and neutral) load onto different dimensions, which is not the case for other components of the GHI. Moreover, a formal Chi squared test speaks in favor of excluding the component—it can be shown to add noise. However, it bears noting that when we use the original sub-indices of the GHI in the regressions reported in Online Appendix-Table 5 (OA, p. 10), including this component, the results are very similar for the GHI public opinion sub-index compared to those found for our favorable opinion sub-index, which excludes the component, as reported in Table 4.

  16. Mobility restrictions may be of a legal kind or follow from either poverty or cultural or family values. Thus, if mobility is not an option, gay men probably do not even entertain the idea of moving. But even when mobility is an option, gay men may have no desire to move, e.g., if the actual or perceived opportunities to lead a good life elsewhere are not any better.

  17. There are two basic types of discrimination: taste-based and statistical (Becker 1957; Phelps 1972). The former is the result of a personal dislike of a particular person for some characteristic; the latter stems from imperfect information and the negative evaluation of a person for belonging to a group that on average is thought to possess negative qualities. In the case of gay men, a dislike may, e.g., be based on prejudice (Herek 2000), disgust (Olatunji 2008) or self-hatred (Weinstein et al. 2012). Our findings suggest that society-level factors influence the degree to which personal (taste-based or statistical) discriminatory motives turn into actually perceived discrimination.

  18. It could also be that the survey responses from these countries are not very representative, since there is an average of 54 replies per country, and that the relationship is therefore not reflecting real general circumstances. As described above we use weights to alleviate the potential small sample problem. In the sensitivity analysis we also present results when excluding countries with <100 observations.

  19. Specifically, all three areas matter for the quality of life of gay men, but for different outcomes. ILGA persecution is related to favorable opinion, absence of discrimination, absence of threats and absence of bad behavior; ILGA protection to the absence of discrimination (not surprising) and absence of threats; and ILGA recognition to favorable opinion, absence of discrimination and the absence of bad behavior. Section 2 of the Online Appendix (OA, p. 3) provides information on the specific coding of these sub-indices.

  20. Readers should note that while the interaction term per se cannot be interpreted on its own—one has to interpret the conditional effects when gauging the size and significance of the results—its significance informs about whether the difference between the estimates in the low- and high-GDP subsamples is significant. As such, the subsample estimates can differ in two ways: (1) either by being significantly different, such that one is clearly larger than the other; or (2) by not being significantly different but where one estimate is substantially more precise when one conditional estimate is significant and the other is not.

  21. We calculate the size of the full effects by, for example, multiplying the estimates of ILGA rights on opinion and discrimination with the effects of opinion and discrimination on life satisfaction and adding the two effects. In the case there is a direct effect, this is added to the sum of the indirect effects to get the full effect.

  22. Since the unique data only includes responses from gay men, we cannot estimate the magnitude of the effects on life satisfaction in comparison to the average population. Moreover, the GHI is not measured in exactly the same way as traditional life-satisfaction indicators, which further complicates direct comparisons of magnitudes.

References

  • Ahmed, A. M., Andersson, L., & Hammarstedt, M. (2013). Are gay men and lesbians discriminated against in the hiring process? Southern Economic Journal, 79, 565–585.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alexander, A. C., Inglehart, R., & Welzel, C. (2015). Emancipating sexuality: Breakthroughs into a bulwark of tradition. Social Indicators Research. doi:10.1007/s11205-015-1137-9.

  • American Psychological Association. (2008). Sexual orientation and homosexuality. http://web.archive.org/web/20130808032050/http://www.apa.org/helpcenter/sexual-orientation.aspx.

  • Becker, G. S. (1957). The economics of discrimination. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berggren, N., & Nilsson, T. (2013). Does economic freedom foster tolerance? Kyklos, 66, 177–207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berggren, N., & Nilsson, T. (2014). Market institutions bring tolerance, especially where there is social trust. Applied Economics Letters, 21, 1234–1237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berggren, N., & Nilsson, T. (2015). Globalization and the transmission of social values: The case of tolerance. Journal of Comparative Economics, 43, 371–389.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bilz, K., & Nadler, J. (2014). Law, moral attitudes and behavioral change. In E. Zamir & D. Taichman (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of behavioral economics and the law (pp. 241–267). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjørnskov, C. (2007). Determinants of generalized trust: A cross-country comparison. Public Choice, 130, 1–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bjørnskov, C., Dreher, A., & Fischer, J. A. V. (2007). The bigger the better? Evidence of the effect of government size on life satisfaction around the world. Public Choice, 130, 267–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bjørnskov, C., Dreher, A., & Fischer, J. A. V. (2008). Cross-country determinants of life satisfaction: Exploring different determinants across groups in society. Social Choice and Welfare, 30, 119–173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bjørnskov, C., Dreher, A., & Fischer, J. A. V. (2010). Formal institutions and subjective well-being: Revisiting the cross-country evidence. European Journal of Political Economy, 26, 419–430.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bjørnskov, C., & Méon, P.-G. (2013). Is trust the missing root of institutions, education, and development? Public Choice, 157, 641–669.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bjørnskov, C., & Tsai, M.-C. (2015). How do institutions affect happiness and misery? A tale of two tails. Comparative Sociology, 14, 353–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blazer, G., & Palmore, E. (1976). Religion and aging in a longitudinal panel. Gerontologist, 16, 82–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boeri, T., Patacchini, E., & Peri, G. (Eds.). (2015). Unexplored dimensions of discrimination. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brambor, T., Clark, W. R., & Golder, M. (2006). Understanding interaction models: Improving empirical analysis. Political Analysis, 14, 63–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buchanan, J. M., & Congleton, R. D. (1998). Politics by principle, not interest: Towards nondiscriminatory democracy. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Case, A., & Deaton, A. (2015). Suicide, age, and well-being: An empirical investigation. In D. A. Wise (Ed.), Insights into the economics of ageing. Chicago: Chicago University Press.

  • Cheibub, J. A., Gandhi, J., & Vreeland, J. R. (2010). Democracy and dictatorship revisited. Public Choice, 143, 67–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • CIA. (2015). The CIA world factbook. Langley, VA: Central Intelligence Agency.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. E., Frijters, P., & Shields, M. A. (2008). Relative income, happiness, and utility: An explanation for the Easterlin paradox and other puzzles. Journal of Economic Literature, 46, 95–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corneo, G., & Jeanne, O. (2009). A theory of tolerance. Journal of Public Economics, 93, 691–702.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deaton, A. (2008). Income, health, and well-being around the world: Evidence from the Gallup World Poll. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 22, 53–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Di Tella, R., MacCulloch, R. J., & Oswald, A. J. (2003). The macroeconomics of happiness. Review of Economics and Statistics, 85, 809–827.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dixit, A. K., & Stiglitz, J. E. (1977). Monopolistic competition and optimal product diversity. American Economic Review, 67, 297–308.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dolan, P., Peasgood, T., & White, M. (2008). Do we really know what makes us happy? A review of the economic literature on the factors associated with subjective well-being. Journal of Economic Psychology, 29, 94–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Donadio, R. (2013). On gay priests, pope Francis asks, “Who am I to judge?” New York Times, July 29.

  • Dorn, D., Fischer, J. A. V., Kirchgässner, G., & Sousa-Poza, A. (2007). Is it culture or democracy? The impact of democracy and culture on happiness. Social Indicators Research, 82, 505–526.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Doyle, D. M., & Molix, L. (2014). Perceived discrimination and well-being in gay men: The protective role of behavioural identification. Psychology & Sexuality, 5, 117–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dreher, A. (2006). Does globalization affect growth? Evidence from a new index of globalization. Applied Economics, 38, 1091–1110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duberman, M., Vicinus, M., & Chauncey, G, Jr (Eds.). (1990). Hidden from history: Reclaiming the gay and lesbian past. New York: Meridian.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fidrmuc, J., & Tunalı, Ç. B. (2015). Happiness and religion. CESifo working paper no. 5437. Munich: CESifo.

  • Frey, B. S., & Stutzer, A. (2002). Happiness and economics: How the economy and institutions affect human well-being. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frey, B. S., & Stutzer, A. (2012). The use of happiness research for public policy. Social Choice and Welfare, 38, 659–674.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fricker, R. D., & Schonlau, M. (2002). Advantages and disadvantages of Internet research surveys: Evidence from the literature. Field Methods, 14, 347–367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, B. (2005). The moral consequences of growth. New York: Alfred A. Knopf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallup. (2014). Gallup world poll. www.gallup.com/consulting/worldpoll/24046/About.aspx.

  • Gates, G. J. (2011). How many people are lesbian, gay, bisexual and transgender? Report. Los Angeles: The Williams Institute, UCLA. http://escholarship.org/uc/item/09h684x2.

  • Glaeser, E. D. (2011). Triumph of the city. New York: Penguin Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gundelach, P., & Kreiner, S. (2004). Happiness and life satisfaction in advanced European countries. Cross-Cultural Research, 38, 359–386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gutmann, J., & Voigt, S. (2015). The rule of law and constitutionalism in Muslim countries. Public Choice, 162, 351–380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hammarstedt, M., Ahmed, A. M., & Aldén, L. (2015). Sexual prejudice and labor market outcomes for gays and lesbians: Evidence from Sweden. Feminist Economics, 21, 90–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heineck, G., & Süssmuth, B. (2013). A different look at Lenin’s legacy: Social capital and risk taking in the two Germanies. Journal of Comparative Economics, 41, 789–803.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Helliwell, J. F., & Huang, H. (2008). How’s your government? International evidence linking good government and well-being. British Journal of Political Science, 38, 595–619.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Helliwell, J. F., Layard, R., & Sachs, J. (2015). World happiness report 2015. New York: The Earth Institute, Columbia University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Helliwell, J. F., & Wang, S. (2011). Trust and wellbeing. International Journal of Wellbeing, 1, 42–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herek, G. M. (2000). The psychology of sexual prejudice. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 19–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heston, A., Summers, R., & Aten, B. (2012). Penn world tables version 7.1. Philadelphia: Center for International Comparisons of Production, Income and Prices, University of Pennsylvania.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hofstede, G. (2001). Culture’s consequences: Comparing values, behaviors, institutions, and organizations across nations (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • ILGA. (2015). State-sponsored homophobia: A world survey of laws: Criminalisation, protection and recognition of same-sex love. Geneva: ILGA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Inglehart, R. (2000). Globalization and postmodern values. Washington Quarterly, 23, 215–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Inglehart, R., & Abramson, P. R. (1999). Measuring postmaterialism. American Political Science Review, 93, 665–677.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaufmann, D., Kraay, A., & Mastruzzi, M. (2009). Governance matters VIII: Aggregate and individual governance indicators, 19962008. World bank policy research working paper no. 4978. Washington, DC: World Bank.

  • Kertzner, R. M., Meyer, I. H., Frost, D. M., & Stirratt, M. J. (2009). Social and psychological well-being in lesbians, gay men, and bisexuals: The effects of race, gender, age, and sexual identity. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 79, 500.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Knack, S., & Keefer, P. (1997). Does social capital have an economic pay-off? Quarterly Journal of Economics, 112, 1251–1288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • KOF. (2015). KOF index of globalization. http://globalization.kof.ethz.ch/.

  • Lemke, R., Tornow, T., & PlanetRomeo.com (2015). Gay happiness monitorResults overview from a global survey on perceived gay-related public opinion and gay well-being. Report. Mainz: Johannes Gutenberg University. www.planetromeo.com/wp-content/uploads/2015/05/GAY_HAPPINESS_MONITOR_2015.pdf.

  • Lim, C., & Putnam, R. D. (2010). Religion, social networks and life satisfaction. American Sociological Review, 75, 914–933.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mondimore, F. M. (1996). A natural history of homosexuality. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mukand, S., & Rodrik, D. (2015). The political economy of liberal democracy. NBER working paper no. 21540. Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic Research.

  • Necker, S., & Voskort, A. (2014). Politics and parents: Intergenerational transmission of values after a regime shift. European Journal of Political Economy, 36, 177–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nussbaum, M. C., & Sen, A. (1993). The quality of life. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Olatunji, B. O. (2008). Disgust, scrupulosity and conservative attitudes about sex: Evidence for a mediational model of homophobia. Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 1364–1369.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Patacchini, E., Ragusa, G., & Zenou, Y. (2015). Unexplored dimensions of discrimination in Europe: Homosexuality and physical appearance. Journal of Population Economics, 28, 1045–1073.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Phelps, E. S. (1972). The statistical theory of racism and sexism. American Economic Review, 62, 659–661.

    Google Scholar 

  • Powdthavee, N., & Wooden, M. (2014). Life satisfaction and sexual minorities: Evidence from Australia and the United Kingdom. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 116, 107–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rawls, J. (1971). A theory of justice. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rode, M. (2013). Do good institutions make citizens happy, or do happy citizens build better institutions? Journal of Happiness Studies, 14, 1479–1505.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stadelmann-Steffen, I., & Vatter, A. (2012). Does satisfaction with democracy really increase happiness? Direct democracy and individual satisfaction in Switzerland. Political Behavior, 34, 535–559.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stevenson, B., & Wolfers, J. (2008). Economic growth and subjective well-being: Reassessing the Easterlin paradox. Brookings Papers on Economic Activity, 39, 1–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tikkanen, R., & Ross, M. W. (2003). Technological tearoom trade: Characteristics of Swedish men visiting gay Internet chat rooms. AIDS Education and Prevention, 15(2), 122–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van den Akker, H., Blaauw, J., Lubbers, M., van der Ploeg, R., Scheepers, P., & Verbakel, E. (2013). Health and happiness among gay couples in European countries 2002–2010. International Psychiatry, 10, 31–33.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weinstein, N., Ryan, W. S., DeHaan, C. R., Przybylski, A. K., Legate, N., & Ryan, R. M. (2012). Parental autonomy support and discrepancies between implicit and explicit sexual identities: Dynamics of self-acceptance and defense. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102, 815–832.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitam, F. L. (1983). Culturally invariable properties of male homosexuality: Tentative conclusions from cross-cultural research. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 12, 207–226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, A., & Laskey, N. (2003). Internet based marketing research: A serious alternative to traditional research methods? Marketing Intelligence & Planning, 21, 79–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wong, C. Y., & Tang, C. S. K. (2003). Personality, psychosocial variables, and life satisfaction of Chinese gay men in Hong Kong. Journal of Happiness Studies, 4, 285–293.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • World Bank. (2015). World development indicators. http://data.worldbank.org/data-catalog/world-development-indicators.

  • World Values Survey (2015). Online database. www.worldvaluessurvey.org.

  • Zellner, A. (1962). An efficient method of estimating seemingly unrelated regressions and tests for aggregation bias. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 57, 348–368.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank Andreas Bergh, Jerg Gutmann, Martin Rode, Reto Odermatt and participants of the 17th Danish Public Choice Workshop (Copenhagen, 2016), the Public Choice Society conference (Fort Lauderdale, 2016) and the European Public Choice Society meeting (Freiburg, 2016) for helpful comments and the Swedish Research Council (Berggren and Nilsson), Torsten Söderberg’s Foundation (Berggren and Nilsson), the Czech Science Foundation (Grant 16-19934S) (Berggren and Nilsson) and the Jan Wallander and Tom Hedelius Foundation (Bjørnskov) for financial support.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Therese Nilsson.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 53 kb)

Appendix

Appendix

Tables 5 and 6 report estimates from which Fig. 4 is constructed.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Berggren, N., Bjørnskov, C. & Nilsson, T. What Aspects of Society Matter for the Quality of Life of a Minority? Global Evidence from the New Gay Happiness Index. Soc Indic Res 132, 1163–1192 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-016-1340-3

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-016-1340-3

Keywords

Navigation