Skip to main content
Log in

What do spelling errors tell us? Classification and analysis of errors made by Greek schoolchildren with and without dyslexia

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In this study we propose a classification system for spelling errors and determine the most common spelling difficulties of Greek children with and without dyslexia. Spelling skills of 542 children from the general population and 44 children with dyslexia, Grades 3–4 and 7, were assessed with a dictated common word list and age-appropriate passages. Spelling errors were classified into broad categories, including phonological (graphophonemic mappings), grammatical (inflectional suffixes), orthographic (word stems), stress assignment (diacritic), and punctuation. Errors were further classified into specific subcategories. Relative proportions for a total of 11,364 errors were derived by calculating the opportunities for each error type. Nondyslexic children of both age groups made primarily grammatical and stress errors, followed by orthographic errors. Phonological and punctuation errors were negligible. Most frequent specific errors were in derivational affixes, stress diacritics, inflectional suffixes, and vowel historical spellings. Older children made fewer errors, especially in inflectional suffixes. Dyslexic children differed from nondyslexic ones in making more errors of the same types, in comparable relative proportions. Spelling profiles of dyslexic children did not differ from those of same-age children with poor reading skills or of younger children matched in reading and phonological awareness. In conclusion, spelling errors of both dyslexic and nondyslexic children indicate persistent difficulty with internalizing regularities of the Greek orthographic lexicon, including derivational, inflectional, and word (stem) families. This difficulty is greater for children with dyslexia.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Although diagnosis is typically given without formal testing, due to lack of standardized testing batteries at the time of the study, this population is fully characterized in terms of reading, spelling, and cognitive performance, in comparison to the general population. Further discussion of the diagnostic tests and criteria can be found in Protopapas and Skaloumbakas (2007). See also Anastasiou and Polychronopoulou (2009) for diagnostic practices and associated problems.

  2. Validation of the discriminant function against expert judgment for Grade 7 has indicated 99.1 % agreement for cases deemed unambiguous (by two experts); see Protopapas & Skaloumbakas (2007) and Protopapas, Skaloumbakas, & Bali (2008) for the details of these discriminant analyses.

  3. The distinguishing measures included, for Grades 3–4: pseudoword repetition, word reading accuracy, passage reading time, and phoneme deletion (based on Protopapas, Skaloumbakas, & Bali, 2008); for Grade 7: pseudoword reading time, word reading accuracy, word reading time, passage reading time (based on Protopapas & Skaloumbakas, 2007).

  4. Naturally, they differ in age, passage reading (because passages were different in the two grade groups), and Raven’s matrices (because it is a raw score, unstandardized for age).

  5. In Figs. 2 and 3 we report mean proportions because most medians are zero and their display would be less informative. It should be kept in mind, however, that error distributions were highly skewed in many error categories, making patterns of means difficult to interpret directly.

  6. The reading-match group does not seem to depart in any way from the general population of Grades 3–4, so the matching process has not resulted in selection of unrepresentative outliers.

  7. The maximum proportion of stress errors in the passage is lower than for the word list because the passage included monosyllabic words, written without a diacritic. A stress error on those would have to be an extraneous diacritic. Such errors were rare, vastly outnumbered by omissions.

  8. The two words are παιδάκια (/pεðaca/ “kids”), with 143 occurrences (4.84 per million tokens), and παϊδάκια (/paiðaca/ “chops”), with 16 occurrences (0.54 per million).

References

  • Aidinis, A., & Nunes, T. (2001). The role of different levels of phonological awareness in the development of reading and spelling in Greek. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 14, 145–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anastasiou, D., & Polychronopoulou, S. (2009). Identification and overidentification of specific learning disabilities (dyslexia) in Greece. Learning Disability Quarterly, 32, 55–69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Angelelli, P., Judica, A., Spinelli, D., Zoccolotti, P., & Luzzatti, C. (2004). Characteristics of writing disorders in Italian dyslexic children. Cognitive & Behavioral Neurology, 17, 18–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Angelelli, P., Notarnicola, A., Judica, A., Zoccolotti, P., & Luzzatti, C. (2010). Spelling impairments in Italian dyslexic children: Phenomenological changes in primary school. Cortex, 46, 1299–1311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernstein, S. E. (2009). Phonology, decoding, and lexical compensation in vowel spelling errors made by children with dyslexia. Reading and Writing, 22, 207–331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. M. (1985). Spelling ability in congenital dysarthria: Evidence against articulatory coding in translating between graphemes and phonemes. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 2, 229–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bourassa, D., & Treiman, R. (2003). Spelling in children with dyslexia: Analyses from the Treiman-Bourassa early spelling test. Scientific Studies of Reading, 7, 309–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bourassa, D., Treiman, R., & Kessler, B. (2006). Use of morphology in spelling by children with dyslexia and typically developing children. Memory & Cognition, 34, 703–714.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruck, M., & Treiman, R. (1990). Phonological awareness and spelling in normal children and dyslexics: The case of initial consonant clusters. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 50, 156–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruck, M., & Waters, G. S. (1988). An analysis of the spelling errors of children who differ in their reading and spelling skills. Applied Psycholinguistics, 9, 77–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, P., Nunes, T., & Aidinis, A. (1999). Different morphemes, same spelling problems: Cross-linguistic developmental studies. In M. Harris & G. Hatano (Eds.), Learning to read and write: A cross-linguistic perspective (pp. 112–133). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  • Caravolas, M. (2004). Spelling development in alphabetic writing systems: A cross-linguistic perspective. European Psychologist, 9, 3–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caravolas, M., Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. J. (2001). The foundations of spelling ability: Evidence from a 3-year longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language, 45, 751–774.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caravolas, M., & Volín, J. (2001). Phonological spelling errors among dyslexic children learning a transparent orthography: The case of Czech. Dyslexia, 7, 229–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caravolas, M., Volín, J., & Hulme, C. (2005). Phoneme awareness is a key component of alphabetic literacy skills in consistent and inconsistent orthographies: Evidence from Czech and English children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 107–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cassar, M., Treiman, R., Moats, L., Pollo, T. C., & Kessler, B. (2005). How do the spellings of children with dyslexia compare with those of nondyslexic children? Reading and Writing, 18, 27–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chliounaki, K., & Bryant, P. (2002). Construction and learning to spell. Cognitive Development, 17, 1489–1499.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chliounaki, K., & Bryant, P. (2007). How children learn about morphological spelling rules. Child Development, 78, 1360–1373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Curtin, S., Manis, F. R., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2001). Parallels between the reading and spelling deficits of two subgroups of developmental dyslexics. Reading and Writing, 14, 515–547.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deacon, S. H., Conrad, N., & Pacton, S. (2008). A statistical learning perspective on children’s learning about graphotactic and morphological regularities in spelling. Canadian Psychology, 49, 118–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Defior, S., Jiménez-Fernández, G., & Serrano, F. (2009). Complexity and lexicality effects on the acquisition of Spanish spelling. Learning and Instruction, 19, 55–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Defior, S., & Serrano, F. (2005). The initial development of spelling in Spanish: From global to analytical. Reading and Writing, 18, 81–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diakogiorgi, K., Baris, T., & Valmas, T. (2005). Ικανότητα χρήσης μορφολογικών στρατηγικών στην ορθογραφημένη γραφή από μαθητές της Α′ τάξης του δημοτικού. [Ability to use morphological strategies in spelling by pupils in 1st elementary grade]. Psychologia, 12, 568–586.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diamanti, V. (2005). Dyslexia and dysgraphia in Greek in relation to normal development: Cross-linguistic and longitudinal studies. Unpublished PhD dissertation, Department of Human Communication Science, University College London.

  • Douklias, S. D., Masterson, J., & Hanley, J. R. (2010). Surface and phonological developmental dyslexia in Greek. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 26, 705–723.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friend, A., & Olson, R. K. (2008). Phonological spelling and reading deficits in children with spelling disabilities. Scientific Studies of Reading, 12, 90–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Furnes, B., & Samuelsson, S. (2010). Predicting reading and spelling difficulties in transparent and opaque orthographies: A comparison between Scandinavian and US/Australian children. Dyslexia, 16, 119–142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Georgiou, G. K., Manolitsis, G., Nurmi, J.-E., & Parrila, R. (2010). Does task-focused versus task-avoidance behavior matter for literacy development in an orthographically consistent language? Contemporary Educational Psychology, 35, 1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Georgiou, G. K., Parrila, R., & Papadopoulos, T. C. (2008). Predictors of word decoding and reading fluency across languages varying in orthographic consistency. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 566–580.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Green, L., McCutchen, D., Schwiebert, C., Quinlan, T., Eva-Wood, A., & Juelis, J. (2003). Morphological development in children’s writing. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 752–761.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gutiérrez-Palma, N., Raya-García, M., & Palma-Reyes, A. (2009). Detecting stress patterns is related to children’s performance on reading tasks. Applied Psycholinguistics, 30, 1–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, M., & Giannouli, V. (1999). Learning to read and spell in Greek: The importance of letter knowledge and morphological awareness. In M. Harris & G. Hatano (Eds.), Learning to read and write: A cross-linguistic perspective (pp. 51–70). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  • Hoefflin, G., & Franck, J. (2005). Development of spelling skills in children with and without learning disabilities. L1–Educational Studies in Language and Literature, 5, 175–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holliman, A. J., Wood, C., & Sheehy, K. (2010). Does speech rhythm sensitivity predict children’s reading ability 1 year later? Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 356–366.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jiménez, J. E., O’Shanahan, I., Tabraue, M., Artiles, C., Muñeton, M., Guzmán, R., et al. (2008). Evolución de la escritura de palabras de ortografía arbitraria en lengua española [Development of spelling words with arbitrary orthography in the Spanish language]. Psicothema, 20, 786–794.

    Google Scholar 

  • Justicia, F., Defior, S., Pelegrina, S., & Martos, F. J. (1999). The sources of error in Spanish writing. Journal of Research in Reading, 22, 198–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Katzir, T., Kim, Y., Wolf, M., Kennedy, B., Lovett, M., & Morris, R. (2006). The relationship of spelling recognition, RAN, and phonological awareness to reading skills in older poor readers and younger reading-matched controls. Reading and Writing, 19, 845–872.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kemp, N., Parrila, R. K., & Kirby, J. R. (2008). Phonological and orthographic spelling in high-functioning adult dyslexics. Dyslexia, 15, 105–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keuning, J., & Verhoeven, L. (2008). Spelling development throughout the elementary grades: The Dutch case. Learning and Individual Differences, 18, 459–470.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landerl, K., & Wimmer, H. (2000). Deficits in phoneme segmentation are not the core problem of dyslexia: Evidence from English and German Children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21, 243–263.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landerl, K., & Wimmer, H. (2008). Development of word reading fluency and spelling in a consistent orthography: An 8-year follow-up. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 150–161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lennox, C., & Siegel, L. (1996). The development of phonological rules and visual strategies in average and poor spellers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 62, 60–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loizidou-Ieridou, N., Masterson, J., & Hanley, J. R. (2010). Spelling development in 6–11-year-old Greek-speaking Cypriot children. Journal of Research in Reading, 33, 247–262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moats, L. C. (1983). A comparison of the spelling errors of older dyslexic and second-grade normal children. Annals of Dyslexia, 33, 121–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moats, L. C. (1995). Spelling: Development, disability, and instruction. Baltimore, MD: York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, H. D. (1980). Analysis of spelling errors in normal and dyslexic children. In U. Frith (Ed.), Cognitive processes in spelling (pp. 475–493). Toronto: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nikolopoulos, D., Goulandris, N., Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. J. (2006). The cognitive bases of learning to read and spell in Greek: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 94, 1–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nikolopoulos, D., Goulandris, N., & Snowling, M. J. (2003). Developmental dyslexia in Greek. In N. Goulandris (Ed.), Dyslexia in different languages: Cross-linguistic comparisons (pp. 53–67). London: Whurr.

    Google Scholar 

  • Notenboom, A., & Reitsma, P. (2003). Investigating the dimensions of spelling ability. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 63, 1039–1059.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nunes, T., Aidinis, A., & Bryant, P. (2006). The acquisition of written morphology in Greek. In R. M. Joshi & P. G. Aaron (Eds.), Handbook of orthography and literacy (pp. 201–218). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nunes, T., Bryant, P., & Bindman, M. (1997). Morphological spelling strategies: Developmental stages and processes. Developmental Psychology, 33, 637–649.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pacton, S., & Deacon, S. H. (2008). The timing and mechanisms of children’s use of morphological information in spelling: A review of evidence from English and French. Cognitive Development, 23, 339–359.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pacton, S., & Fayol, M. (2003). How do French children use morphosyntactic information when they spell adverbs and present participles? Scientific Studies of Reading, 7, 273–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pacton, S., Perruchet, P., Fayol, M., & Cleeremeans, A. (2001). Implicit learning out of the lab: The case of orthographic regularities. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 401–426.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Papadopoulos, T. C., Georgiou, G. K., & Kendeou, P. (2009). Investigating the double-deficit hypothesis in Greek: Findings from a longitudinal study. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 42, 528–547.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Porpodas, C. D. (1999). Patterns of phonological and memory processing in beginning readers and spellers of Greek. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32, 406–416.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Porpodas, C. D. (2001). Cognitive processes in first grade reading and spelling of Greek. Psychologia, 8, 384–400.

    Google Scholar 

  • Protopapas, A. (2006). On the use and usefulness of stress diacritics in reading Greek. Reading & Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 19, 171–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Protopapas, A., & Gerakaki, S. (2009). Development of processing stress diacritics in reading Greek. Scientific Studies of Reading, 13, 453–483.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Protopapas, A., Gerakaki, S., & Alexandri, S. (2007). Sources of information for stress assignment in reading Greek. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 695–720.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Protopapas, A., & Skaloumbakas, C. (2007). Computer-based and traditional screening and diagnosis of reading disabilities in Greek. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 40, 15–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Protopapas, A., & Skaloumbakas, C. (2010). Κατηγοριοποίηση ορθογραφικών λαθών [Classification of spelling errors]. In A. Mouzaki & A. Protopapas (Eds.), Ορθογραφία: Μάθηση και διαταραχές [Spelling: learning and disorders] (pp. 67–104). Athens: Gutenberg.

    Google Scholar 

  • Protopapas, A., Skaloumbakas, C., & Bali, P. (2008). Validation of unsupervised computer-based screening for reading disability in the Greek elementary Grades 3 and 4. Learning Disabilities: A Contemporary Journal, 6, 45–69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Protopapas, A., Tzakosta, M., Chalamandaris, A., & Tsiakoulis, P. IPLR: An online resource for Greek word-level and sublexical information. Language Resources & Evaluation (in press). doi:10.1007/s10579-010-9130-z

  • Protopapas, A., & Vlahou, E. L. (2009). A comparative quantitative analysis of Greek orthographic transparency. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 991–1008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sawyer, D. J., Wade, S., & Kim, J. K. (1999). Spelling errors as a window on variations in phonological deficits among students with dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 49, 137–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seymour, P. H. K., Aro, M., & Erskine, J. M. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 143–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sideridis, G. D., Mouzaki, A., Protopapas, A., & Simos, P. (2008). Ψυχομετρική διερεύνηση μιας ορθογραφικής δοκιμασίας για τους μαθητές του δημοτικού σχολείου [Psychometric evaluation of a spelling test for elementary school students]. Psychologia, 15, 290–315.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, L. S., Share, D., & Geva, E. (1995). Evidence for superior orthographic skills in dyslexics. Psychological Science, 6, 250–254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tangel, D. M., & Blachman, B. A. (1992). Effect of phoneme awareness instruction on kindergarten children’s invented spelling. Journal of Literacy Research, 24, 233–261.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tangel, D. M., & Blachman, B. A. (1995). Effect of phoneme awareness instruction on the invented spelling of first grade children: A one year follow-up. Journal of Reading Behavior, 27, 153–185.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R. (1993). Beginning to spell: A study of first-grade children. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R., & Bourassa, D. (2000). Children’s written and oral spelling. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21, 183–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R., Broderick, V., Tincoff, R., & Rodriguez, K. (1998). Children’s phonological awareness: Confusions between phonemes that differ only in voicing. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 68, 3–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R., Cassar, M., & Zukowski, A. (1994). What types of linguistic information do children use in spelling? The case of flaps. Child Development, 65, 1318–1337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waters, G. S., Bruck, M., & Malus-Abramovitz, M. (1988). The role of linguistic and visual information in spelling: A developmental study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 45, 400–421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H., & Landerl, K. (1997). How learning to spell German differs from learning to spell English. In C. A. Perfetti, L. Rieben, & M. Fayol (Eds.), Learning to spell: Research, theory, and practice across languages (pp. 81–96). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahos, G. I., & Zahos, D. I. (1998). Δυσλεξία: Αντιμετώπιση-Αποκατάσταση. Οδηγίες εφαρμογής προγράμματος [Dyslexia: Guide to the application of an intervention-remediation program]. Athens: Author.

Download references

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Fani Sakellaropoulou for help in spelling error classification and to Vassiliki Diamanti, George K. Georgiou, and Panagiotis G. Simos for comments on the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Athanassios Protopapas.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (PDF 543 kb)

Appendix

Appendix

See Table 5.

Table 5 Opportunities for spelling errors: definitions and counts

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Protopapas, A., Fakou, A., Drakopoulou, S. et al. What do spelling errors tell us? Classification and analysis of errors made by Greek schoolchildren with and without dyslexia. Read Writ 26, 615–646 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-012-9378-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-012-9378-3

Keywords

Navigation