Skip to main content
Log in

Individual differences in trait anxiety and goal-commitment predict updating efficiency on the reading span task

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

According to attentional control theory (ACT; Eysenck et al. in Emotion 7(2):336–353, 2007) anxious individuals recruit motivation on demanding tasks, which helps prevent performance shortfalls. We used a quasi-experimental design to examine the relationship between trait anxiety (operationalised using questionnaire scores), situational stress (manipulated using ego threat instructions) and motivation (indexed using a self-report goal-commitment scale) in predicting effectiveness (accuracy) and efficiency (accuracy divided by RT) on the reading span task. After controlling for depression, the variables were not related to effectiveness; however there was a significant trait anxiety × goal-commitment interaction on reading span efficiency. Higher trait anxiety predicted better efficiency at higher goal-commitment, and poorer efficiency at lower goal-commitment, and these relationships were independent of situational stress. Results are interpreted in terms of ACT.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ansari, T. L., Derakshan, N., & Richards, A. (2008). Effects of anxiety on task switching: Evidence from the mixed anti-saccade task. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 229–238. doi:10.3758/CABN.8.3.229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barlow, D. (2000). Unraveling the mysteries of anxiety and its disorders from the perspective of emotion theory. American Psychologist, 55, 1245–1263. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.55.11.1247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berggren, N., & Derakshan, N. (2013). Attentional control deficits in trait anxiety: Why you see them and why you don’t. Biological Psychology, 92(3), 440–446. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.03.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, T. A., Chorpita, B. F., Korotitsch, W., & Barlow, D. H. (1997). Psychometric properties of the depression anxiety stress scale (DASS) in clinical samples. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35(1), 79–89. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(96)00068-X.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Calvo, M. (1985). Effort, aversive, representations and performance in test anxiety. Personality and Individual Differences, 6, 563–571.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Calvo, M. G., Ramos, P. M., & Estevez, A. (1992). Test anxiety and comprehension efficiency: The role of prior knowledge and working memory deficits. Anxiety, Stress and Coping, 5, 125–138. doi:10.1080/10615809208250492.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conway, A. R. A., Kane, M. J., Bunting, M. F., Hambrick, D. Z., Wilhelm, O., & Engle, R. W. (2005). Working memory span tasks: A methodological review and user’s guide. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(5), 769–786. doi:10.3758/BF03196772.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 450–466. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(80)90312-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Darke, S. (1988). Anxiety and working memory capacity. Cognition and Emotion, 2, 145–154. doi:10.1080/02699938808408071.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Derakshan, N., Ansari, T. L., Hansard, M., Shoker, L., & Eysenck, M. W. (2009). Anxiety, inhibition, efficiency, and effectiveness: An investigation using the antisaccade task. Experimental Psychology, 56(1), 48–55. doi:10.1027/1618-3169.56.1.48.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Derakshan, N., & Eysenck, M. W. (2009). Anxiety, processing efficiency, and cognitive performance: New developments from attentional control theory. European Psychologist, 14(2), 168–176. doi:10.1027/1016-9040.14.2.168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, M. S., Burt, J. S., & Lipp, O. V. (2006). Selective processing of masked and unmasked verbal threat material in anxiety: Influence of an immediate acute stressor. Cognition and Emotion, 20(6), 812–835. doi:10.1080/02699930500375761.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, E. J., Edwards, M. S., & Lyvers, M. (2015a). Cognitive trait anxiety, situational stress and mental effort predict shifting efficiency: Implications for attentional control theory. Emotion, 15(3), 350–359. doi:10.1037/emo0000051.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, E. J., Edwards, M. S., & Lyvers, M. (2016a). Inter-relationships between trait anxiety, situational stress and mental effort predict phonological processing efficiency, but not effectiveness. Emotion. doi:10.1037/emo0000138.

  • Edwards, M. S., Edwards, E. J., & Lyvers, M. (2016b). Cognitive trait anxiety, stress and effort interact to predict inhibitory control. Cognition and Emotion. doi:10.1080/02699931.2016.1152232.

  • Edwards, M. S., Moore, P., Champion, J. C., & Edwards, E. J. (2015b). Effects of trait anxiety and situational stress on attentional shifting are buffered by working memory capacity. Anxiety, Stress and Coping, 28(1), 1–16. doi:10.1080/10615806.2014.911846.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, M. W. (1985). Anxiety and cognitive-task performance. Personality and Individual Differences, 6, 579–586. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(85)90007-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, M. W., & Calvo, M. G. (1992). Anxiety and performance: The processing efficiency theory. Cognition and Emotion, 6(6), 409–434. doi:10.1080/02699939208409696.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, M. W., & Derakshan, N. (2011). New perspectives in attentional control theory. Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 955–960. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.08.019.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., & Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion, 7(2), 336–353. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.336.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fales, C. L., Barch, D. M., Burgess, G. C., Schaefer, A., Mennin, D. S., Gray, J. R., et al. (2008). Anxiety and cognitive efficiency: Differential modulation of transient and sustained neural activity during a working memory task. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 8(3), 239–253. doi:10.3758/CABN.8.3.239.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gros, D. F., Antony, M. M., Simms, L. J., & McCabe, R. E. (2007). Psychometric properties of the state-trait inventory for cognitive and somatic anxiety (STICSA): Comparison to the state-trait anxiety inventory (STAI). Psychological Assessment, 19(4), 369–381. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.19.4.369.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, L. M., & Cumming, S. R. (2003). An examination of the relationship between anxiety and performance on prospective and retrospective memory tasks. Australian Journal of Psychology, 55(1), 51–55. doi:10.1080/00049530412331312874.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harvey, P. O., Le Bastard, G., Pochon, J. B., Levy, R., Allilaire, J. F., Dubois, B., et al. (2004). Executive functions and updating of the contents of working memory in unipolar depression. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 38(6), 567–576. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2004.03.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A., MacLeod, C., & Hammond, G. (2009). Anxiety-linked task performance: Dissociating the influence of restricted working memory capacity and increased investment of effort. Cognition and Emotion, 23(4), 753–781. doi:10.1080/02699930802131078.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Humphreys, M. S., & Revelle, W. (1984). Personality, motivation, and performance: A theory of the relationship between individual differences and information processing. Psychological Review, 91, 153–184. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.91.2.153.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jaros, S. (2009). Measurement of commitment. In H. J. Klein, T. E. Becker, & J. P. Meyer (Eds.), Commitment in organizations. Accumulated wisdom and new directions (pp. 347–381). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klein, H., Wesson, M. J., Hollenback, J. R., Wright, P. M., & DeShon, R. P. (2001). The Assessment of goal commitment: A measurement model meta-analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 85(1), 32–55. doi:10.1006/obhd.2000.2931.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting and task performance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lovibond, S. H., & Lovibond, P. F. (1995). Manual for the depression anxiety and stress scales (2nd ed.). Sydney: Psychology Foundation of Australia.

    Google Scholar 

  • Masson, M. E., & Miller, J. A. (1983). Working memory and individual differences in comprehension and memory of text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 75(2), 314–318. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.75.2.314.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meier, L. L. (2008). IRSE: Interactions in multiple linear regression with SPSS and Excel (Version1.6) [Computer software and manual]. Retrieved April 2, 2014, from http://www.urenz/irse.

  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100. doi:10.1006/cogp.1999.0734.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pacheco-Unguetti, A. P., Acosta, A., Lupiáñez, J., Román, N., & Derakshan, N. (2012). Response inhibition and attentional control in anxiety. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(4), 646–660. doi:10.1080/17470218.2011.637114.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ree, M. J., French, D., MacLeod, C., & Locke, V. (2008). Distinguishing cognitive and somatic dimensions of state and trait anxiety: Development and validation of the State-Trait Inventory for Cognitive and Somatic Anxiety (STICSA). Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 36(03), 313–332. doi:10.1017/S1352465808004232.

  • Sarason, I. G. (1984). Stress, anxiety and cognitive interference: Reactions to tests. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 929–938. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.46.4.929.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sorg, B. A., & Whitney, P. (1992). The effect of trait anxiety and situational stress on working memory capacity. Journal of Research in Personality, 26, 235–241. doi:10.1016/0092-6566(92)90041-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vergara, C., & Roberts, J. E. (2011). Motivation and goal orientation in vulnerability to depression. Cognition and Emotion, 25(7), 1281–1290. doi:10.1080/02699931.2010.542743.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vytal, K., Cornwell, B., Arkin, N., & Grillon, C. (2012). Describing the interplay between anxiety and cognition: From impaired performance under low cognitive load to reduced anxiety under high load. Psychophysiology, 49(6), 842–852. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01358.x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Walkenhorst, E., & Crowe, S. F. (2009). The effect of state worry and trait anxiety on working processes in a normal sample. Anxiety, Stress and Coping, 22, 167–187. doi:10.1080/10615800801998914.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wong, I. Y., Mahar, D., Titchener, K., & Freeman, J. (2013). The impact on processing efficiency: Implications for attentional control theory. The Open Behavioral Science Journal, 7(6), 7–15. doi:10.2174/1874230001307010007.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by an Australian Post-graduate Award Scholarship and a Queensland Smart Futures Top-up Scholarship awarded to Elizabeth Edwards. We acknowledge the contribution of Michael Richter and an anonymous reviewer who helped us to improve this manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Elizabeth J. Edwards.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Edwards, E.J., Edwards, M.S. & Lyvers, M. Individual differences in trait anxiety and goal-commitment predict updating efficiency on the reading span task. Motiv Emot 40, 936–945 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-016-9572-8

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-016-9572-8

Keywords

Navigation