Skip to main content
Log in

The Role of the Ugly = Bad Stereotype in the Rejection of Misshapen Produce

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Business Ethics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

A substantial portion of produce harvested around the world is wasted because it does not meet consumers’ shape expectations. Only recently has research begun investigating the causes underlying misshapen produce rejection by consumers. Generally, this limited research has concluded that misshapen produce is subject to an ugly penalty, leading consumers to form biased expectations regarding product attributes (e.g., healthiness, tastiness, or naturalness). In this research, we propose that this ugly penalty extends to the moral valuation of misshapen produce and that this moral penalty is rooted in the feelings of disgust evoked by the visual appearance of such produce. Across five studies and two pilot studies, we show that misshapen fruits and vegetables are disgust elicitors, and that felt disgust causes consumers to make negative moral associations with produce which, in turn, leads to decreased preference. We also show that pairing misshapen produce with a positive unconditioned stimulus (i.e., an image of attractive individuals) is an efficient solution to counteract the effect of this stereotype. This research contributes to the growing body of work demonstrating the link between consumption behaviors, consumer preferences, and morality.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In this research, we refer to non-misshapen produce as normally-shaped, in that these are the shapes that consumers normally (typically) expect and represent the market norms (or standards) for what the produce should look like. This is the universally adopted definition in extant literature. For example, Mookerjee et al. (2021) define misshapen produce as “that which has a significant natural aesthetic deviation in shape and/or color from prototypical produce, but has no damage or disease that could affect safety, taste, or nutrition” (p. 63). Misshapen may operate on a continuum where some shape differences are minor and un-noticeable, but we assume that these are not sorted out because they are too similar to prototypical produce, whereas some shape deviations are very obvious. While these more obvious deviations are likely sorted out before reaching the consumer, in the case that they are presented on shelves, consumers are more likely to notice them.

  2. To clarify the differences between the three types of individual disgust traits, Tybur et al (2009) provide the following definitions: “pathogen disgust, which motivates the avoidance of infectious microorganisms; sexual disgust, which motivates the avoidance of sexual partners and behaviors that would jeopardize one’s long-term reproductive success; and moral disgust, which motivates the avoidance of social norm violator” (p. 103). Importantly, sensitivity to pathogen disgust is related to “infectious agents, including dead bodies, rotting foods, and bodily fluids such as feces, phlegm, vomit, blood, and semen, and it motivates proximal avoidance of such things” (p. 105) which it not at all related to misshapen produce, which we have conceptually outlined as being free of rot, bruising, or other indicators of decay. Furthermore, we argue that the disgust elicited by misshapen produce is non-pathogen related, and more aesthetically driven. This is also further shown in our Pilot Study.

  3. A similar message is used by Kromkommer: https://www.kromkommer.com/english/

  4. A similar message is used by Rubies in the Rubbel: https://rubiesintherubble.com/

  5. Intermarché in France used this tactic: https://www.theglobeandmail.com/report-on-business/industry-news/marketing/dont-judge-a-fruit-by-how-ugly-it-is/article19876009/

  6. As variances of the five groups were different for self-evaluation and feelings of disgust, we confirmed those findings with Mann–Whitney non-parametric tests. The two tests yielded convergent results.

  7. One participant did not provide their age or gender.

References

  • Amar, M., Ariely, D., Carmon, Z., & Yang, H. (2018). How counterfeits infect genuine products: The role of moral disgust. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 28(2), 329–343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Argo, J. J., Dahl, D. W., & Morales, A. C. (2006). Consumer contamination: How consumers react to products touched by others. Journal of Marketing, 70(2), 81–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aschemann-Witzel, J., De Hooge, I., Amani, P., Bech-Larsen, T., & Oostindjer, M. (2015). Consumer-related food waste: Causes and potential for action. Sustainability, 7(6), 6457–6477.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Avramova, Y. R., & Inbar, Y. (2013). Emotion and moral judgment. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 4(2), 169–178.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bazzini, D., Curtin, L., Joslin, S., Regan, S., & Martz, D. (2010). Do animated disney characters portray and promote the beauty–goodness stereotype? Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 40(10), 2687–2709.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bentley, S. (2011). Who’s to blame for supermarket rejection of ‘ugly’ fruit and vegetables? Retrieved April 14, 2018, from https://theecologist.org/2011/dec/29/whos-blame-supermarket-rejection-ugly-fruit-and-vegetables

  • Bloch, P. H. (1995). Seeking the ideal form: Product design and consumer response. Journal of Marketing, 59(3), 16–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brough, A. R., Wilkie, J. E., Ma, J., Isaac, M. S., & Gal, D. (2016). Is eco-friendly unmanly? The green-feminine stereotype and its effect on sustainable consumption. Journal of Consumer Research, 43(4), 567–582.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, M. C., & Winterich, K. P. (2018). A framework for the consumer psychology of morality in the marketplace. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 28, 167–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, T. P., Pogacar, R., Pullig, C., Kouril, M., Aguilar, S., LaBouff, J., Isenberg, N., & Chakroff, A. (2019). Survey-software implicit association tests: A methodological and empirical analysis. Behavior Research Methods, 51(5), 2194–2208.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chan, C., Van Boven, L., Andrade, E. B., & Ariely, D. (2014). Moral violations reduce oral consumption. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 24(3), 381–386.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Chapman, H. A., & Anderson, A. K. (2013). Things rank and gross in nature: A review and synthesis of moral disgust. Psychological Bulletin, 139(2), 300.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chapman, H. A., Kim, D. A., Susskind, J. M., & Anderson, A. K. (2009). In bad taste: Evidence for the oral origins of moral disgust. Science, 323(5918), 1222–1226.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Culpepper, P. D., Havlíček, J., Leongómez, J. D., & Roberts, S. C. (2018). Visually activating pathogen disgust: A new instrument for studying the behavioral immune system. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1397.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Curtis, V., & de Barra, M. (2018). The structure and function of pathogen disgust. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society b: Biological Sciences, 373(1751), 20170208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Bock, T., Pandelaere, M., & Van Kenhove, P. (2013). When colors backfire: The impact of color cues on moral judgment. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 23(3), 341–348.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Hooge, I. E., Oostindjer, M., Aschemann-Witzel, J., Normann, A., Loose, S. M., & Almli, V. L. (2017). This apple is too ugly for me!: Consumer preferences for suboptimal food products in the supermarket and at home. Food Quality and Preference, 56, 80–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeCoster, J., Banner, M. J., Smith, E. R., & Semin, G. R. (2006). On the inexplicability of the implicit: Differences in the information provided by implicit and explicit tests. Social Cognition, 24(1), 5–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Desmet, P. (2002). Designing emotions. Delft University of Technology, Department of Industrial Design.

    Google Scholar 

  • Devin, B., & Richards, C. (2018). Food waste, power, and corporate social responsibility in the Australian food supply chain. Journal of Business Ethics, 150(1), 199–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dion, K., Berscheid, E., & Walster, E. (1972). What is beautiful is good. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 24(3), 285–290.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H., Ashmore, R. D., Makhijani, M. G., & Longo, L. C. (1991). What is beautiful is good, but…: A meta-analytic review of research on the physical attractiveness stereotype. Psychological Bulletin, 110(1), 109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Egolf, A., Siegrist, M., & Hartmann, C. (2018). How people’s food disgust sensitivity shapes their eating and food behaviour. Appetite, 127, 28–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eskine, K. J. (2013). Wholesome foods and wholesome morals? Organic foods reduce prosocial behavior and harshen moral judgments. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 4(2), 251–254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eskine, K. J., Kacinik, N. A., & Prinz, J. J. (2011). A bad taste in the mouth gustatory disgust influences moral judgment. Psychological Science, 22(3), 295–299.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Feeding America. (2021). Facts about poverty and hunger in America. Retrieved March 13, 2021, from https://www.feedingamerica.org/hunger-in-america/facts

  • Feinberg, M., Kovacheff, C., Teper, R., & Inbar, Y. (2019). Understanding the process of moralization: How eating meat becomes a moral issue. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 117(1), 50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fessler, D. M. T., Arguello, A. P., Mekdara, J. M., & Macias, R. (2003). Disgust sensitivity and meat consumption: A test of an emotivist account of moral vegetarianism. Appetite, 41(1), 31–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • FitzGerald, C., Martin, A., Berner, D., & Hurst, S. (2019). Interventions designed to reduce implicit prejudices and implicit stereotypes in real world contexts: A systematic review. BMC Psychology, 7(1), 29.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Forscher, P. S., Lai, C. K., Axt, J. R., Ebersole, C. R., Herman, M., Devine, P. G., & Nosek, B. A. (2019). A meta-analysis of procedures to change implicit measures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 117(3), 522.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • French, A. R., Franz, T. M., Phelan, L. L., & Blaine, B. E. (2013). Reducing Muslim/Arab stereotypes through evaluative conditioning. The Journal of Social Psychology, 153(1), 6–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Galli, F., Cavicchi, A., & Brunori, G. (2019). Food waste reduction and food poverty alleviation: A system dynamics conceptual model. Agriculture and Human Values, 36(2), 289–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Galoni, C., Carpenter, G. S., & Rao, H. (2020). Disgusted and afraid: Consumer choices under the threat of contagious disease. Journal of Consumer Research, 47(3), 373–392.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gershon, R., & Cryder, C. (2018). Goods donations increase charitable credit for low-warmth donors. Journal of Consumer Research, 45(2), 451–469.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Godoy, M. (2016). Wal-mart, America’s largest grocer, is now selling ugly fruit and vegetables. NPR The Salt.

  • Gorn, G. J. (1982). The effects of music in advertising on choice behavior: A classical conditioning approach. Journal of Marketing, 46(1), 94–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graham, J., Haidt, J., & Nosek, B. A. (2009). Liberals and conservatives rely on different sets of moral foundations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(5), 1029–1046.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 1464.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grewal, L., Hmurovic, J., Lamberton, C., & Reczek, R. W. (2019). The self-perception connection: Why consumers devalue unattractive produce. Journal of Marketing, 83(1), 89–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Griffin, A. M., & Langlois, J. H. (2006). Stereotype directionality and attractiveness stereotyping: Is beauty good or is ugly bad? Social Cognition, 24(2), 187–206.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hagen, L. (2020). Pretty healthy food: How and when aesthetics enhance perceived healthiness. Journal of Marketing, 0022242920944384.

  • Hagen, L. (2021). Pretty healthy food: How and when aesthetics enhance perceived healthiness. Journal of Marketing, 85(2), 129–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J., & Kesebir, S. (2010). Morality. In S. T. Fiske, D. T. Gilbert, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (pp. 797–832). John Wiley & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J., McCauley, C., & Rozin, P. (1994). Individual differences in sensitivity to disgust: A scale sampling seven domains of disgust elicitors. Personality and Individual Differences, 16(5), 701–713.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A. F. (2017). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. Guilford Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hickey, M. E., & Ozbay, G. (2014). Food waste in the united states: A contributing factor toward environmental instability. Frontiers in Environmental Science, 2, 51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hingston, S. T., & Noseworthy, T. J. (2020). On the epidemic of food waste: Idealized prototypes and the aversion to misshapen fruits and vegetables. Food Quality and Preference, 86, 103999.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hollands, G. J., Prestwich, A., & Marteau, T. M. (2011). Using aversive images to enhance healthy food choices and implicit attitudes: An experimental test of evaluative conditioning. Health Psychology, 30(2), 195.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Horberg, E. J., Oveis, C., Keltner, D., & Cohen, A. B. (2009). Disgust and the moralization of purity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(6), 963.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Inbar, Y., Pizarro, D. A., Knobe, J., & Bloom, P. (2009). Disgust sensitivity predicts intuitive disapproval of gays. Emotion, 9(3), 435.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, A., & Fitness, J. (2008). Moral hypervigilance: The influence of disgust sensitivity in the moral domain. Emotion, 8(5), 613–627.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kader, A. A. (2002). Fruits in the global market fruit quality and its biological basis (pp. 1–16). Sheffield Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kim, J., Allen, C. T., & Kardes, F. R. (1996). An investigation of the mediational mechanisms underlying attitudinal conditioning. Journal of Marketing Research, 33(3), 318–328.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y. (2017). Consumer responses to the food industry’s proactive and passive environmental CSR, factoring in price as CSR tradeoff. Journal of Business Ethics, 140(2), 307–321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klebl, C., Luo, Y., & Bastian, B. (2021). Beyond aesthetic judgment: Beauty increases moral standing through perceptions of purity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 01461672211023648.

  • Krashinsky, S. (2014). Ugly fruit campaign prompts consumers to rethink what they buy. July 31. Retrieved March 13, 2021, from https://www.theglobeandmail.com/report-on-business/industry-news/marketing/dont-judge-a-fruit-by-how-ugly-it-is/article19876009/

  • Kumar, M., & Garg, N. (2010). Aesthetic principles and cognitive emotion appraisals: How much of the beauty lies in the eye of the beholder? Journal of Consumer Psychology, 20(4), 485–494.

    Article  MathSciNet  Google Scholar 

  • Lai, C. K., Marini, M., Lehr, S. A., Cerruti, C., Shin, J.-E.L., Joy-Gaba, J. A., Ho, A. K., Teachman, B. A., Wojcik, S. P., Koleva, S. P., Frazier, R. S., Heiphetz, L., Chen, E. E., Turner, R. N., Haidt, J., Kesebir, S., Hawkins, C. B., Schaefer, H. S., Rubichi, S., et al. (2014). Reducing implicit racial preferences: I. A comparative investigation of 17 interventions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(4), 1765.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Langlois, J. H., & Styczynski, L. E. (1979). The effects of physical attractiveness on the behavioral attributions and peer preferences of acquainted children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 2(4), 325–341.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lebens, H., Roefs, A., Martijn, C., Houben, K., Nederkoorn, C., & Jansen, A. (2011). Making implicit measures of associations with snack foods more negative through evaluative conditioning. Eating Behaviors, 12(4), 249–253.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loebnitz, N., & Grunert, K. G. (2018). The impact of abnormally shaped vegetables on consumers’ risk perception. Food Quality and Preference, 63, 80–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loebnitz, N., Schuitema, G., & Grunert, K. G. (2015). Who buys oddly shaped food and why? Impacts of food shape abnormality and organic labeling on purchase intentions. Psychology & Marketing, 32(4), 408–421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martijn, C., Sheeran, P., Wesseldijk, L. W., Merrick, H., Webb, T. L., Roefs, A., & Jansen, A. (2013). Evaluative conditioning makes slim models less desirable as standards for comparison and increases body satisfaction. Health Psychology, 32(4), 433.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mason, E. C., & Richardson, R. (2012). Treating disgust in anxiety disorders. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 19(2), 180.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mookerjee, S., Cornil, Y., & Hoegg, J. (2019). Stating the obvious: How “ugly” labels can increase purchase of unappealing produce and reduce waste. ACR North American Advances.

  • Mookerjee, S., Cornil, Y., & Hoegg, J. (2021). From waste to taste: How “ugly” labels can increase purchase of unattractive produce. Journal of Marketing, 85(3), 62–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moreda, G. P., Muñoz, M. A., Ruiz-Altisent, M., & Perdigones, A. (2012). Shape determination of horticultural produce using two-dimensional computer vision—A review. Journal of Food Engineering, 108(2), 245–261.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2006). Reducing automatically activated racial prejudice through implicit evaluative conditioning. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(4), 421–433.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Orth, U. R., & Malkewitz, K. (2008). Holistic package design and consumer brand impressions. Journal of Marketing, 72(3), 64–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Park, J. H., Schaller, M., & Crandall, C. S. (2007). Pathogen-avoidance mechanisms and the stigmatization of obese people. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28(6), 410–414.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Philipp-Muller, A., Teeny, J. D., & Petty, R. E. (2022). Do consumers care about morality? A review and framework for understanding morality’s marketplace influence. Consumer Psychology Review, 5(1), 107–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pizarro, D. A., Inbar, Y., & Helion, C. (2011). On disgust and moral judgment. Emotion Review, 3(3), 267–268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Powell, P. A., Jones, C. R., & Consedine, N. S. (2019). It’s not queasy being green: The role of disgust in willingness-to-pay for more sustainable product alternatives. Food Quality and Preference, 78, 103737.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Royte, E. (2016). How ‘ugly’ fruits and vegetables can help solve world hunger. March. Retrieved September 12, 2016, from http://www.nationalgeographic.com/magazine/2016/03/global-food-waste-statistics/

  • Rozin, P., & Fallon, A. (1980). The psychological categorization of foods and non-foods: A preliminary taxonomy of food rejections. Appetite, 1(3), 193–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rozin, P., & Fallon, A. E. (1987). A perspective on disgust. Psychological Review, 94(1), 23.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rozin, P., & Haidt, J. (2013). The domains of disgust and their origins: Contrasting biological and cultural evolutionary accounts. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(8), 367–368.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rozin, P., Millman, L., & Nemeroff, C. (1986). Operation of the laws of sympathetic magic in disgust and other domains. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(4), 703.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rozin, P., & Nemeroff, C. (1990). The laws of sympathetic magic: A psychological analysis of similarity and contagion. In J. W. Stigler, R. A. Schweder, & G. Herdt (Eds.), Cultural psychology: Essays on comparative human development. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rozin, P., & Singh, L. (1999). The moralization of cigarette smoking in the united states. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 8(3), 321–337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rozin, P., Spranca, M., Krieger, Z., Neuhaus, R., Surillo, D., Swerdlin, A., & Wood, K. (2004). Preference for natural: Instrumental and ideational/moral motivations, and the contrast between foods and medicines. Appetite, 43(2), 147–154.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45(3), 513.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scott, S. E., Inbar, Y., & Rozin, P. (2016). Evidence for absolute moral opposition to genetically modified food in the united states. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(3), 315–324.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scott, S. E., & Rozin, P. (2020). Actually, natural is neutral. Nature Human Behaviour, 4, 1–2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sherman, G. D., & Clore, G. L. (2009). The color of sin: White and black are perceptual symbols of moral purity and pollution. Psychological Science, 20(8), 1019–1025.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Suher, J., Szocs, C., & van Ittersum, K. (2021). When imperfect is preferred: The differential effect of aesthetic imperfections on choice of processed and unprocessed foods. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 49, 1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sweldens, S., Van Osselaer, S. M., & Janiszewski, C. (2010). Evaluative conditioning procedures and the resilience of conditioned brand attitudes. Journal of Consumer Research, 37(3), 473–489.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Townsend, C., & Shu, S. B. (2010). When and how aesthetics influences financial decisions. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 20(4), 452–458.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tsukiura, T., & Cabeza, R. (2011). Remembering beauty: Roles of orbitofrontal and hippocampal regions in successful memory encoding of attractive faces. NeuroImage, 54(1), 653–660.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tybur, J. M., Lieberman, D., & Griskevicius, V. (2009). Microbes, mating, and morality: Individual differences in three functional domains of disgust. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(1), 103–122.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Leeuwen, F., Dukes, A., Tybur, J. M., & Park, J. H. (2017). Disgust sensitivity relates to moral foundations independent of political ideology. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, 11(1), 92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Overveld, W., De Jong, P. J., Peters, M. L., Cavanagh, K., & Davey, G. C. (2006). Disgust propensity and disgust sensitivity: Separate constructs that are differentially related to specific fears. Personality and Individual Differences, 41(7), 1241–1252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • VanderWaal, D. (2017). If it is unsightly, is it less healthy? Understanding the consumer’s desire for aesthetically perfect produce.

  • Vartanian, L. R., Thomas, M. A., & Vanman, E. J. (2013). Disgust, contempt, and anger and the stereotypes of obese people. Eating and Weight Disorders-Studies on Anorexia, Bulimia and Obesity, 18(4), 377–382.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vartanian, L. R., Trewartha, T., & Vanman, E. J. (2016). Disgust predicts prejudice and discrimination toward individuals with obesity. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 46(6), 369–375.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vermeir, I., & Roose, G. (2020). Visual design cues impacting food choice: A review and future research agenda. Foods, 9(10), 1495.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Walsh, E. M., & Kiviniemi, M. T. (2014). Changing how i feel about the food: Experimentally manipulated affective associations with fruits change fruit choice behaviors. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 37(2), 322–331.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Waytz, A., Iyer, R., Young, L., Haidt, J., & Graham, J. (2019). Ideological differences in the expanse of the moral circle. Nature Communications, 10(1), 1–12.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wezel, A., Herren, B. G., Kerr, R. B., Barrios, E., Gonçalves, A. L. R., & Sinclair, F. (2020). Agroecological principles and elements and their implications for transitioning to sustainable food systems. A Review. Agronomy for Sustainable Development, 40(6), 1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wheatley, T., & Haidt, J. (2005). Hypnotic disgust makes moral judgments more severe. Psychological Science, 16(10), 780–784.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zellner, D. A., Loss, C. R., Zearfoss, J., & Remolina, S. (2014). It tastes as good as it looks! The effect of food presentation on liking for the flavor of food. Appetite, 77, 31–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, J. W., Piff, P. K., Iyer, R., Koleva, S., & Keltner, D. (2014). An occasion for unselfing: Beautiful nature leads to prosociality. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 37, 61–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhong, C.-B., & Liljenquist, K. (2006). Washing away your sins: Threatened morality and physical cleansing. Science, 313(5792), 1451–1452.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

This study was funded by a research grant from a research chair internal to the school of the corresponding author.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Nathalie Spielmann.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors have no competing interests to declare.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Animal Welfare

This article does not contain any studies with animals performed by any of the authors.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study when applicable. In online studies, participants were informed of their anonymity, did not reveal personal details and cannot have their answers traced back to them. All respondents could opt out when they wished. In the field study, participants were not informed of their participation, but no personal information was collected and there was no forced participation.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendices

Appendix A: Study Stimuli

Study 1: IAT (Normally Shaped Produce on the Left, Misshapen Produce on the Right)

figure a

Study 1: Explicit Measures

figure b

Study 2: Cherry

figure c

Study 3A: Ad Appeals (with Translations)

figure d
figure e
figure f

Posters for Study 3B

figure g

Appendix B: Pilot Study: Pathogens and Misshapen Produce

We conducted an additional study to test whether disgust influencing misshapen produce rejection is derived from the belief that it conveys pathogens. A sample composed of 84 French students was recruited in a business school to participate in a lab study (42.2% Male; Mage = 20.00 years old,Footnote 7SD = 0.80) and used a one-factor, two-level (tomato shape: misshapen vs. normally shaped) between-subjects design. Respondents were presented with one of the four tomatoes on a paper plate (top two tomatoes were the misshapen tomatoes and the bottom two were the normally shaped tomatoes), as seen below. The puncture wound on the top right tomato occurred after the study.

figure h

We measured purchase likelihood by asking respondents how likely they would be to buy the tomato on a scale of 1 = I would never buy this tomato to 7 = I would definitely buy this tomato. We then measured perceived pathogen risk with two items (“It is highly likely that this tomato contains harmful substances” and “This tomato is tainted”; r = 0.25, p < 0.05).

Mean comparisons revealed that the misshapen tomato was less likely to be purchased than the normally shaped tomato (Mmisshapen = 4.78, SD = 1.85; Mnormal = 5.53, SD = 1.37, F(1,82) = 4.49, p < 0.05. However, respondents did not perceive a higher risk of contamination or pathogens for the misshapen tomato vs the normally shaped tomato (Mmisshapen = 2.64, SD = 1.20; Mnormal = 3.01, SD = 1.35, F(1,82) = 1.78, p = 0.186).

Appendix C: Pilot Study: Tastiness, Healthiness, Morality, Disgust, and Misshapen Produce

As outlined by Mookerjee et al. (2021) taste and health expectations are signals of quality for food and they do not measure quality directly. As such, and to adequately compare our results to those of Mookerjee et al. (2021), we used measures from their research as quality and compared these to our core concepts: disgust and morality, both in a parallel mediation as well as placing them as covariates.

Method

A sample of 139 US respondents (48% Female; Mage = 36.17 years old, SD = 12.40) was recruited through an online panel managed by Prolific. Participants were randomly assigned to one of two conditions (standard lemon or misshapen lemon) in which they were invited to imagine that they were buying groceries from an online retailer. First, participants were exposed to a ficticious ad by an imagined grocer.

figure i

Respondents were then told to imagine they had made an online order with the e-grocer and their order included fruits and vegetables, and specifically a lemon. When they received and unpacked their order, they take the lemon out of the shopping bag and then they were presented with one of the two lemons (standard or misshapen).

figure j

Respondents were then asked what would be the probability that they would actually consume or use this lemon to make something to eat or drink using a single item seven-point bipolar scale anchored with improbable to very probable. Respondents then answered the three-item seven-point morality scale used in studies 1 and 2 (α = 0.84). They were then asked to rate their disgust emotional state on a scale of 1 = does not describe my feelings to 7 = clearly describes my feelings; we averaged the reported scores for disgust and nauseated as an index (r = 0.79, p < 0.001). We adapted a tastiness measure from Mookerjee et al. (2021) by having respondents answer how much they believed the lemon they received was tasty, flavorful, and delicious and they answered using a seven-point likert scale anchored with 1 = strongly disagree and 7 = strongly agree (α = 0.97). Likewise, we also used an adapted measure from Mookerjee et al. (2021) to measure perceived healthiness by having respondents answer how much they believed that the lemon was healthy, nutritional, and full of vitamins using a seven-point likert scale anchored with 1 = strongly disagree and 7 = strongly agree (α = 0.96). Finally, we asked respondents to tell us what they thought of the lemon using the same five-item manipulation check as in our previous studies (α = 0.97).

Results

Manipulation check. A one factor ANOVA with the condition (standard or misshapen lemon) as the factor and the aggregated manipulation check measure as the dependent variable revealed a significant effect of the condition (F(1,137) = 281.58, p < 0.001). Consumers faced with the misshapen lemon were less likely to want to consume it than those presented with the standard lemon (Mmisshapen = 2.53, SD = 1.47 vs. Mstandard = 6.12, SD = 1.02).

Main effects. A one factor ANOVA with the condition (standard or misshapen lemon) as the factor and consumption intentions as the dependent variable revealed a significant effect of the condition (F(1,137) = 25.81, p < 0.001). Consumers faced with the misshapen lemon were less likely to want to consume it than those presented with the standard lemon (Mmisshapen = 5.22, SD = 1.98 vs. Mstandard = 6.56, SD = 0.96). We found the same pattern of effects for morality, disgust, tastiness, and healthiness. Consumers perceived the misshapen lemon as less moral (F(1,137) = 60.48, p < 0.001; Mmisshapen = 4.36, SD = 1.26 vs. Mstandard = 5.83, SD = 0.95), more disgusting (F(1,137) = 38.79, p < 0.001; Mmisshapen = 2.51, SD = 1.43 vs. Mstandard = 1.26, SD = 0.86), less tasty (F(1,137) = 57.47, p < 0.001; Mmisshapen = 4.71, SD = 1.35 vs. Mstandard = 6.12, SD = 0.77), and less healthy (F(1,137) = 31.36, p < 0.001; Mmisshapen = 5.15, SD = 1.24 vs. Mstandard = 6.12, SD = 0.78).

Parallel mediation analysis. In the subsequent analyses, we contrasted all the proposed mediators (morality, disgust, tastiness, and healthiness) with the condition (standard vs misshapen lemon) as the independent variable and consumption likelihood as the dependent variable. The parallel mediation models (using Model 4 from PROCESS, Hayes, 2013) showed that perceived product morality mediated the effect of the lemon condition on consumption intentions (b =  − 0.37, SE = 0.18, 95%CI [− 0.7377, − 0.0365]). Disgust was also a significant mediator (b =  − 0.31, SE = 0.16, 95%CI [− 0.6477, − 0.0048]). Taste was a significant mediator (b =  − 0.67, SE = 0.27, 95%CI [− 1.2309, − 0.1531]) but healthiness was not (b =  − 0.27, SE = 0.17, 95%CI [− 0.6247, 0.0454]). The direct effect was not significant (b = 0.27, SE = 0.23, 95%CI [− 0.1731, 0.7185]).

Serial mediation analysis. As originally proposed in our theorizing, we tested the serial mediation effect of disgust and morality on consumption intentions. That is, misshapen produce leads to higher levels of disgust (b = 1.24, SE = 0.20, p < 0.001), disgust leads to lowered levels of morality (b =  − 0.40, SE = 0.07, p < 0.001), and lowered morality leads to decreased consumption intentions (b = 0.61, SE = 0.10, p < 0.001). The indirect effect is significant (b =  − 0.30, SE = 0.10, 95%CI [− 0.5256, − 0.1402]) and the direct effect is not significant (b = 0.11, SE = 0.25, 95%CI [− 0.3823, 0.5982]).

Discussion

Overall, this study demonstrates that morality plays an important role in determining consumption rejection of misshapen produce, and this even when accounting for tastiness and healthiness perceptions. Healthiness was not found to be a significant mediator. We expected tastiness to be a significant mediator as taste, disgust, morality, and consumption are intricately related. Consumers are less likely to consume foods they evaluate as less tasty, and often disgusting and foods deemed as having negative moral associations are perceived as having less taste, which explains lowered consumption intentions. The opposite is also true – tastier foods are perceived as less disgusting and more moral. Importantly, taste, disgust, and morality are theoretically linked (Eskine, 2013; Eskine et al., 2011). We also find this in our results when conducting the serial mediation of misshapen produce → disgust → morality → taste → consumption intentions and find a significant indirect effect (b =  − 0.17, SE = 0.06, 95%CI [− 0.3122, − 0.0694]) and non-significant direct effect (b = 0.31, SE = 0.23, 95%CI [− 0.1346, 0.7591]), thereby confirming the role of moral associations in the relationship between disgust and taste for misshapen produce.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Spielmann, N., Gomez, P. & Minton, E. The Role of the Ugly = Bad Stereotype in the Rejection of Misshapen Produce. J Bus Ethics 190, 413–437 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-023-05420-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-023-05420-1

Keywords

Navigation